Expertise can be defined as
"the perceived ability of the source to make valid assertions" (McCracken
1989, p. 311). It describes the extent to which the source is able to provide
valid information (Hovland et al., 1953). This extent can be determined by
assessing source’s aspects, such as such as knowledge, experience, or skills
(Erdogan, 1999). Farr (2007) propose that expertise can be examined in two
ways, basing on its two main characteristics, technical and practical
competence (Braunsberger & Munch, 1998, p.24). Technical competence
concerns the skilfulness of the source and its possession of special knowledge
(Braunsberger & Munch, 1998. p.24). It can be acquired by “training,
reading, formal education and scholarly pursuits’’ (Farr, 2007, p. 244). As
opposed to technical competence, practical competence refers to the experience
and skills possessed from direct participations in activates and can be
achieved through trial and error process and practise (Braunsberger &
Munch, 1998, p.24). Farr & White (2003) showed that both technical and
practical competences contribute to the evaluation of perceived expertise. When
a source has little practical competence, the technical competence had a small
effect, however when a source is high in practical competence, technical
competence was found to have a significant effect on the perceived credibility (Farr,
2007, p.244).
However, self-proclaimed experts
and real experts could present comparable level of knowledge and be perceived
by consumers as similar. It means that an online reviewer who is a professional
in a given field could be seen as having the same degree of expertise as a
self-taught man. Particularly in the context of online environment, consumers
may have difficulties to assess the expertise of an online reviewer. Many times
the Internet anonymity makes it impossible to identify reviewer’s qualifications,
because reviewers commonly use nicknames instead of their real names and
provide scarce information about their background (Chang & Chuang, 2011,
p.16; Zhao et al., 2015, p.1347). However, recipients can evaluate the
reviewer’s expertise in a certain topic of interest by evaluating cues that
signal expertise, such as the specific knowledge or strength of arguments
embedded in the message (Clark et al., 2012, p.91; Cosenza et al., 2015, p.73).
Source expertise was found to
have a significant influence on information adoption (Cheung et al., 2008). The
information source that is high in expertise is more persuasive than a source
low in expertise (Maddux & Rogers, 1980). Thus, information sources
perceived as “experts” will be more influential than non-expert ones
(Braunsberger & Munch, 1998, p.25). Moreover, a source high in expertise
can positively influence the attitude towards the source (Maddux & Rogers,
1980; McGinnies & Ward 1980), attitude towards the message (Homer & Kahle,
1990), and attitude toward information usefulness (Gunawan, 2015; Sussman &
Siegal, 2003). Furthermore, it also reported that source expertise influences
consumers’ attitude, behavioural intention and actual behaviour (McGinnies
& Ward, 1980). Therefore, source expertise is a crucial element of the
information persuasiveness (Dholakia & Sternthal, 1977). The measurement
used to evaluate source expertise was adopted from Ohanian (1990) and included
3 items, namely, “expert”, “knowledgeable” and “qualified”. These attributes
were chosen because they are considered to be clear indicators of expertise as
well as have high loadings (Ohanian, 1990, p.43).