Loading...

Messages

Proposals

Stuck in your homework and missing deadline? Get urgent help in $10/Page with 24 hours deadline

Get Urgent Writing Help In Your Essays, Assignments, Homeworks, Dissertation, Thesis Or Coursework & Achieve A+ Grades.

Privacy Guaranteed - 100% Plagiarism Free Writing - Free Turnitin Report - Professional And Experienced Writers - 24/7 Online Support

A bigram detector fires in response to the

10/11/2020 Client: papadok01 Deadline: 3 days

chapter 4

Recognizing Objects

Recognition: Some Early Considerations You're obviously able to recognize a huge number of different patterns-different objects (cats, cups, coats), various actions (crawling, climbing, clapping), and different sorts of situations (crises, comedies). You can also recognize many variations of each of these things. You recognize cats standing up and cats sitting down, cats running and cats asleep. And the same is true for recognition of most other patterns in your recognition repertoire.

You also recognize objects even when the available information is incomplete. For example, you can still recognize a cat if only its head and one paw are visible behind a tree. You recognize a chair though the person blocks much of the chair from view recognize tens of thousands of words, and you can even when someone is sitting on it, even All of this is true for print as well. You can recognize them whether the words are printed in large type or small, italics or straight letters, UPPER CASE or lower. You can even recognize handwritten words, for which the variation from one to the next is huge. These variations in the "stimulus input" provide our first indication that object recognition complexity. Another indication comes from the fact that your recognition of various or otherwise, is influenced by the context in which you encounter those objects. involves some objects, print Consider Figure 4.3. The middle character is the same in both words, but the character looks more like an H in the word on the left and more like an A in the word on the right. With this, you easily read the word on the left as "THE" and not "TAE" and the word on the right as "CAT" and not "CHT." Of course, object recognition is powerfully influenced by the stimulus itself-that is, by the features that are in view. Processes directly shaped by the stimulus are sometimes called "data driven" but are more commonly said to involve bottom-up processing. The effect of context, however, reminds us that recognition is also influenced by one's knowledge and expectations. As a result, your reading of Figure 4.3 is guided by your knowledge that "THE" and "CAT" are common words but that "TAE" and "CHT" are not. This sort of influence-relying on your knowledge-is sometimes called "concept- driven," and processes shaped by knowledge are said to involve top-down processing.

What mechanism underlies both the top- down and bottom -up influences? In the next section, we'll consider a classic proposal for what the mechanism might be. We'll then build on this base as we discuss more recent elaborations of this proposal. The Importance of Features Common sense suggests that many objects can be recognized by virtue of their parts. You recognize an elephant because you see the trunk, the thick legs, the large body. You know a lollipop is a lollipop because you see the circle shape on top of the straight stick. But how do you recognize the parts themselves? How, for example, do you recognize the trunk on the elephant or the circle in the lollipop? The answer may be simple: Perhaps you recognize the parts by looking at their parts-such as the arcs that make up the circle in the lollipop, or the (roughly) parallel lines that identify the elephant's trunk.

To put this more generally, recognition might begin with the identification of visual features in the input pattern- the vertical lines, curves, diagonals, and so on. With these features appropriately catalogued, you can start assembling the larger units. If you detect a horizontal together with a vertical, you know you're looking at a right angle; if you've detected four right angles, you know you're looking at a square.

This broad proposal lines up well with the neuroscience evidence we discussed in Chapter 3 There, we saw that specialized cells in the visual system do seem to act as feature detectors, firing (producing an action potential) whenever the relevant input (i.e. the appropriate feature) is in view. Also, we've already noted that people can recognize many variations on the objects they encounter- cats in different positions, A's in different fonts or different handwritings. An emphasis on features, though, might help with this point. The various A's, for example, differ from one another in overall shape, but they do have certain things in common: two inwardly sloping lines and a horizontal crossbar. Focusing on features, therefore, might allow us to concentrate on elements shared by the various A's and so might allow us to recognize A's despite their apparent diversity. The importance of features is also evident in data from visual search tasks -tasks in which are asked to examine a display and to judge whether a particular target is present in the or not. This search is remarkably efficient when someone is searching for a target defined by participants display a simple feature-for example, finding a vertical segment in a field of horizontals or a green shape a field of red shapes. But people in are generally slower in searching for a target defined as a combination of features (see Figure 4.4). This is just what we would expect if feature analysis is an early step in your analysis of the visual world-and separate from the step in which you combine the features you've detected. Further support for these claims comes from studies of brain damage. At the start of the chapter, we mentioned apperceptive agnosia-a disorder that involves an inability to assemble the various aspects of an input into an organized whole. A related disorder, integrative agnosia, derives from damage to the parietal lobe. Patients with this disorder appear relatively normal in tasks requiring them simply them to judge how the features are bound together to form complex objects. (See, for example, markedly impaired in tasks that require display, but they are to detect features in a Behrmann, Peterson, Moscovitch, & Suzuki, 2006; Humphreys & Riddoch, 2014; Robertson, Treisman, Friedman-Hill, & Grabowecky, 1997. For related results, in which transcranial magnetic stimulation was used to disrupt portions of the brain in healthy individuals, see Ashbridge, Walsh, & Cowey, 1997.) e. Demonstration 4.1: Features and Feature Combination The chapter discusses the importance of features in your recognition of objects, and this priority of features is, in fact, easy to demonstrate. In each of the squares below, find the target. In the square can you find... Most people find the search in Square A (finding the O) to be extremely easy; the search in Square B (finding the L) is harder. Why is this? In Square A, all you need to do is search for the feature "curve" (or "roundness"). That single feature is enough to identify the target, and searching for a single feature is fast and easy. In contrast, the target in Square B is not defined in terms of a single feature. The target (the L) and the distractor items (the T's) have the same features (one horizontal and one vertical); the target is distinguished from the distractors only in how the features are assembled. Therefore, you can't locate the target in Square B simply by hunting for a single feature; instead, you need to take an additional step: You need to think about how the features are put together, and that's a slower, more effortful process.

The data pattern becomes even clearer in Squares C and D. In Square C, the target (again, defined by a single feature) seems to "pop out" at you, and your search in C is just as fast as it was in A. In other words, when hunting for a single feature, you can hunt through 11 items (Square C) as quickly as you can hunt through 4 items (Square A).

In Square D, however, the target doesn't "pop out." Here, you're searching for a feature combination, so you need to examine the forms one by one. As a result, Square D (with 11 shapes to examine) takes more time than Square B (with just 4).

Demonstration adapted from Thornton, T., & Gilden, D. (2007). Parallel and serial processes in visual search. Psychological Review, 114, 71-103. Word Recognition Several lines of evidence, therefore, indicate that object recognition does begin with the detection of simple features. Then, once this detection has occurred, separate mechanisms are needed to put the features together, assembling them into complete objects. But how does this assembly proceed, so that we end up seeing not just the features but whole words-or Chihuahuas, or fire hydrants? In tackling this question, it will be helpful to fill in some more facts that we can then use as a guide to our theory building. Factors Influencing Recognition In many studies, participants have been shown stimuli for just a brief duration-perhaps 20 or 30 ms (milliseconds). Older research did this by means of a tachistoscope, a device designed to present stimuli for precisely controlled amounts of time. More modern research uses computers, but the brief displays are still called "tachistoscopic presentations."

Each stimulus is followed by a post-stimulus mask-often, a random pattern of lines and curves, or a random jumble of letters such as "XJDKEL" The mask interrupts any continued processing that participants might try to do for the stimulus just presented. In this way, researchers can be certain that a stimulus presented for (say) 20 ms is visible for exactly 20 ms and no longer. Can people recognize these briefly visible stimuli? The answer including how familiar a stimulus is. If the stimulus is a word, we can measure familiarity by depends on many factors, counting how often that word appears in print, and these counts are an excellent predictor of tachistoscopic recognition. In one early experiment, Jacoby and Dallas (1981) showed participants words that were either very frequent (appearing infrequent (occurring only 1 to 5 times per million words of print). Participants viewed these words for 35 ms, followed by a mask. Under these circumstances, they recognized almost twice as many of at least 50 times in every million printed words) or the frequent words (see Figure 4.5A). Another factor influencing recognition is recency of view. If participants view a word and then, a little later, view it again, they will recognize the word more readily the second time around. The first exposure primes the participant for the second exposure; more specifically, this is a case of repetition priming.

As an example, participants in one study read a list of words aloud. The participants were then shown a series of words in a tachistoscope. Some of these words were from the earlier list and so had been primed; others were unprimed. For words that were high in frequency, 68 % of the unprimed words were recognized, compared to 84 % of the primed words. For words low in frequency, 37% of the unprimed words were recognized, compared to 73% of the primed words (see Figure 4.5B; Jacoby & Dallas, 1981). The Word-Superiority Effect Figure 4.3 suggests that the recognition of a letter depends on its context-and so an ambiguous letter is read as an A in one setting but an H in another setting. But context also has another effect: Even when a letter is properly printed and quite unambiguous, it's easier to recognize if it appears within a word than if it appears in isolation. This result might seem paradoxical, because here we have a setting in which it seems easier to do "more work" rather than "less"-and so you're more accurate in recognizing all the letters that make up a word (maybe a total of five or six letters) than you are in recognizing just one letter on its own. Paradoxical or not, this pattern is easy to demonstrate, and the advantage for perceiving letters-in- context is called the word-superiority effect (WSE).

The WSE is demonstrated with a "two-alternative, forced-choice" procedure. For example, in some trials we might present a single letter-let's say K-followed by a post-stimulus mask, and follow that with a question: "Which of these was in the display: an E or a K?" In other trials, we might present a word-let's say "DARK"-followed by a mask, followed by a question: "Which of these was in the display: an E or a K?"

Note that participants have a 50-50 chance of guessing correctly in either of these situations, and so any contribution from guessing is the same for the letters as it is for the words. Also, for the word stimulus, both of the letters we've asked about are plausible endings for the stimulus; either ending would create a common word ("DARE" or "DARK"). Therefore, participants who saw only part of the display (perhaps "DAR") couldn't use their knowledge of the language to figure out the display's final letter. In order to choose between E and K, therefore, participants really need to have seen the relevant letter- and that is exactly what we want.

In this procedure, accuracy rates are reliably higher in the word condition. Apparently, recognizing an entire word is easier than recognizing isolated letters (see Figure 4.6; Johnston & McClelland, 1973; Reicher, 1969; Rumelhart & Siple, 1974; Wheeler, 1970). Degree of Well-Formedness As it turns out, though, the term "word-superiority effect" may be misleading, because we don't need words to produce the pattern evident in Figure 4.6. We get a similar effect if we present participants with letter strings like "FIKE" or "LAFE" These letter strings are not English words and they're not familiar, but they look like English strings and (related) are easy to pronounce. And, crucially, strings like these produce a context effect, with the result that letters in these contexts are easier to identify than letters alone.

This effect occurs, though, only if the context is of the right sort. There's no context benefit if we present a string like "HZYE" or "SBNE" An E presented within these strings will not show the word- superiority effect-that is, it won't be recognized more readily than an E presented in isolation.

A parallel set of findings emerge if, instead of asking participants to detect specific letters, ask them to report all of what they have seen. A letter string like "HZYE" is extremely hard to recognize if presented briefly. With a stimulus like this and, say, a 30-ms exposure, participants may report that they only saw a flash and no letters at all; at best, they may report a letter or two. But with the same 30-ms exposure, participants will generally recognize (and be able to report) strings like "FIKE" or "LAFE" although they do even better if the stimuli presented are actual, familiar words.

How should we think about these findings? One approach emphasizes the statistically defined regularities in English spelling. Specifically, we can work through a dictionary, counting how often (for example) the letter combination "FI" occurs, or the combination "LA," or "HZ" We can do the same for three-letter sequences ("FIK," "LAF" and so on). These counts will give us a tally that reveals which letter combinations are more probable in English spelling and which are less probable. We can then use this tally to evaluate new strings-asking, for any string, whether its letter sequences are high-probability ones (occurring often) or low-probability (occurring rarely). These statistical measures allow us to evaluate how "well formed" a letter string is-that is, how well the letter sequence conforms to the usual spelling patterns of English-and well-formedness is a good predictor of word recognition: The more recognize that string, and also the greater the context benefit the string will produce. This well- documented pattern has been known for more than a century (see, e.g., Cattell, 1885) and has been English-like the string is, the easier it will be to replicated in many studies (Gibson, Bishop, Schiff, & Smith, 1964; Miller, Bruner, & Postman, 1954). Making Errors Let's recap some important points. First, it seems that a letter will be easier to recognize if it appears in a well-formed sequence, but not if it appears in a random sequence. Second, well-formed strings perceive than ill-formed strings; this advantage remains even if the well- are, overall, easier to formed strings are made-up ones that you've "COTER"). All using your knowledge of spelling patterns when you look at, and recognize, the words you encounter-and so you have an easier time with letter strings never seen before (strings like "HAKE" or of these facts suggest that you somehow are that conform to these patterns, compared to strings that do not. The influence of spelling patterns is also evident in the mistakes you make. With brief exposures, word recognition is good but not perfect, and the errors that occur are systematic: There's a strong tendency to misread less-common letter sequences as if they were more-common patterns. So, for example, "TPUM" is likely to be misread as "TRUM" or even "DRUM." But the reverse errors are rare: "DRUM" is unlikely to be misread as "TRUM" or "TPUM" These errors can sometimes be quite large-so that someone shown "TPUM" might instead perceive "TRUMPET" But, large or small, the errors show the pattern described: Misspelled words partial words, or nonwords are read in a way that brings them into line with normal spelling. In effect, people perceive the input as being recognition seems to be guided by (or, in this case, misguided by) some knowledge of spelling more regular than it actually is. Once again, therefore, our patterns. e. Demonstration 4.2: The Broad Influence of the Rules of Spelling As the chapter describes, many lines of evidence suggest that we are sensitive to the rules of English spelling: We recognize letter strings more easily if the strings conform to these rules. Likewise strings that obey the spelling rules produce a context effect, but other letter strings do not. In addition, our recognition errors seem to be guided by these rules.

Here's another way to demonstrate how you're influenced by the rules of spelling. First, get a pen and a blank piece of paper ready. Next, start tapping your foot at a speed of roughly one tap per second. Then, with your foot still tapping, read the following list at a speed of taps (roughly 2 seconds) per word. When you're done, close the list, grab the pen, and write down as many of the words as you can remember. HIDE Trap Crown Salt Raise Bore Clean Kite Twist Cord Blend How well did you do? Next, start your foot tapping again-with a speed of two taps per letter string-and do the same task with the following list. HIDE Kero Eevts Wtse Iymts Ookc Rgzea Nmdi Rlmao Kesi Ugdra How well did you do? It's virtually certain that you did less well with the second list, but why? Is it just because you were familiar with the letter strings in the first list (all common words) but totally unfamiliar with the strings in the second list? To test this hypothesis, try the task again with the following list-again, at two taps per letter string. HIDE Fird Teash Buls Blost Vose Shune Tupe Stend Hune Glone How well did you do when the strings are totally unfamiliar but conform to the rules of spelling? Feature Nets and Word Recognition What lies behind this broad pattern of evidence? What are the processes inside of us that lead to the findings we've described? Psychologists' understanding of these points grows out of a theory published many years ago (Selfridge, 1959). Let's start with that theory, and then use it as our base as we look at more modern work. (For a glimpse of some of the modern research, including work that links theorizing to neuroscience, see Carreiras, Armstrong, Perea, & Frost, 2014.) The Design of a Feature Net

Imagine that we want to view. How might design a system that will recognize the word "CLOCK" whenever it is in our "CLOCK" detector work? One option is to "wire" this detector to a C-detector an L-detector, an O-detector, and so on. Then, whenever these letter detectors are activated, this would activate the word detector. But what activates the letter detectors? Maybe the L-detector is "wired" to a horizontal-line detector and also a vertical-line detector, as shown in Figure 4.7. When these feature detectors are activated, this activates the letter detector. The idea is that there could be a network of detectors, organized in layers. The "bottom" layer is concerned with features, and that is why networks of this sort are often called feature nets. As we move "upward" in the network, each subsequent layer is concerned with larger-scale objects; using the term we introduced earlier, the flow of information would be bottom-up-from the lower levels toward the upper levels.

But what does it mean to "activate" a detector? At any point in time, each detector in the network has a particular activation level, which reflects the status of the detector at that moment-roughly how energized the detector is. When a detector receives some input, its activation level increases. A strong input will increase the activation level by a lot, and so will a series of weaker inputs. In either case, the activation level will eventually reach the detector's response threshold, and at that point the detector will fire-that is, send its signal to the other detectors to which it is connected.

These points parallel our description of neurons in Chapter 2, and that's no accident. If the feature net is to be a serious candidate for how humans recognize patterns, then it has to use the same sorts of building blocks that the brain does. However, let's be careful not to overstate this point: No one is suggesting that detectors are neurons or even large groups of neurons. Instead, detectors probably involve complex assemblies of neural tissue. Nonetheless, it's plainly attractive that the hypothesized detectors in the feature net function in a way that's biologically sensible. Within the net, some detectors will be easier to activate than others-that is, some will require a strong input to make them fire, while others will fire even with a weak input. This difference is created in part by how activated each detector is to begin with. If the detector is moderately activated at the start, then only a little input is needed to raise the activation level to threshold, and so it will be easy to make this detector fire. If a detector is not at all activated at the start, then a strong input is needed to bring the detector to threshold, and so it will be more difficult to make this detector fire.

What determines a detector's starting activation level? As one factor, detectors that have fired recently will have a higher activation level (think of it as a "warm-up" effect). In addition, detectors that have fired frequently in the past will have a higher activation level (think of it as an "exercise" effect). Overall, then, activation level is dependent on principles of recency and frequency.

We now can put these mechanisms to work. Why are frequent words in the language easier to recognize than rare words? Frequent words, by definition, appear often in the things you read. Therefore, the detectors needed for recognizing these words have been frequently used, so they have relatively high levels of activation. Thus, even a weak signal (e.g., a brief or dim presentation of the word) will bring these detectors to their response threshold and will be enough to make them fire. As a result, the word will be recognized even with a degraded input.

Repetition priming is explained in similar terms. Presenting a word once will cause the relevant detectors to fire. Once they've fired, activation levels will be temporarily lifted (because of recency of use). Therefore, only a weak signal will be needed to make the detectors fire again. As a result, the word will be more easily recognized the second time around. The Feature Net and Well-Formedness The net we've described so far cannot, however, explain all of the data. Consider the effects of well- formedness-for instance, the fact that people are able to read letter strings like "PIRT" or "HICE" even when those strings are presented very briefly (or dimly or in low contrast), but not strings like explain this finding? One option is to add another layer to the net, a layer filled with detectors for letter conmbinations. Thus, in Figure 4.8, we've added a layer of bigram-detectors of letter pairs. These detectors, like all the rest, will be triggered by lower-level detectors and send their output to higher-level detectors. And just like any other detector, each bigram detector will start out with a certain activation level, influenced by the frequency with which the detector has fired in the past and by the recency with which it has fired. This turns out to be all the theory we need. You have never seen the sequence "HICE" before, but you have seen the letter pair HI (in "HIT," "HIGH," or "HILL") and the pair CE ("FACE," "MICE" "JUICE"). The detectors for these letter pairs, therefore, have high activation levels at the start, so they don't need much additional input to reach their threshold. As a result, these detectors will fire with only weak input. That will make the corresponding letter combinations easy to recognize, facilitating the recognition of strings like "HICE" None of this is true for "IJPV" or "RSFK" Because none of these letter combinations are familiar, these strings will receive no benefits from priming. As a result, a strong input will be needed to bring the relevant detectors to threshold, and so these strings will be recognized only with difficulty. (For more on bigram detectors and how they work see Grainger, Rey, & Dufau, 2008; Grainger & Whitney, 2004; Whitney, 2001. For some complications, see Rayner & Pollatsek, 2011.) Recovery from Confusion Imagine that we present the word "CORN" for just 20 ms. In this setting, the visual system has only a limited opportunity to analyze the input, so it's possible that you'll miss some of the input's features. For example, let's imagine that the second letter in this word-the O-is hard to see, so that only the bottom curve is detected. This partial information invites confusion. If all you know is "the second letter had a bottom maybe an S. Figure 4.9 perhaps it was a U, or a Q, or curve," then perhaps this letter was an O, or shows how this would play out in terms of the network. We've already said that you detected the bottom curve, and that means the "bottom-curve detector" is activated. This detector, in turn provides input to the O-detector and also to the detectors for U, Q, and S, and so activation in this feature detector causes activation in all of these letter detectors. Of course, each of these letter detectors is wired so that it can also receive input from other feature detectors. (And so usually the O-detector also gets input from detectors for left curves, right curves, and top curves.) We've already said, though, that with this brief input these other features weren't detected this time around. As a result, the O-detector will only be weakly activated (because it's not getting its usual full input), and the same is true for the detectors for U. Q, and S.

In this situation, therefore, the network has partial information at the feature level (because only one of the O's features was detected), and this leads to confusion at the letter level: Too many letter detectors are firing (because the now-activated bottom-curve detector is wired to all of them). And, roughly speaking, all of these letter detectors are firing in a fashion that signals uncertainty, because they're each receiving input from only one of their usual feature detectors.

The confusion continues in the information sent upward from the letter level to the bigram level. The detector for the CO bigram will receive a strong signal from the C-detector (because the C was clearly visible) but only a weak signal from the O-detector (because the O wasn't clearly visible). The CU-detector will get roughly the same input-a strong signal from the C-detector and a weak signal from the U-detector. Likewise for the CQ- and CS-detectors. In other words, we can imagine that the signal being sent from the letter detectors is "maybe CO or maybe CU or maybe CQ or maybe CS." The confusion is, however, sorted out at the bigram level. All four bigram detectors in this situation are receiving the same input-a strong signal from one of their letters and a weak signal from the other. But the four detectors don't all respond in the same way. The CO-detector is well primed (because this is a frequent pattern), so the activation it's receiving will probably be enough to fire this (primed) detector. The CU-detector is less primed (because this is a less frequent pattern); the CQ- and CS-detectors, if they even exist, are not primed at all. The input to these latter detectors is therefore unlikely to activate them-because, again, they're less well primed and so won't respond to this weak input.

What will be the result of all this? The network was "under-stimulated" at the feature level (with only a subset of the input's features detected) and therefore confused at the letter level (with too many detectors firing). But then, at the bigram level, it's only the CO-detector that fires, because at this level it is the detector (because of priming) most likely to respond to the weak input. Thus, in a totally automatic fashion, the network recovers from its own confusion and, in this case, avoids an error. Ambiguous Inputs Look again at Figure 4.3. The second character is exactly the same as the fifth, but the left-hand string is perceived as "THE" (and the character is identified as an H) and the right-hand string is perceived as "CAT" (and the character as an A).

What's going on here? In the string on the left, the initial T is clearly in view, and so presumably the T-detector will fire strongly in response. The next character in the display will probably trigger some of the features normally associated with an A and some normally associated with an H. This will cause the A-detector to fire, but only weakly (because only some of the A's features are present), and likewise for the H-detector. At the letter level, then, there will be uncertainty about what this character is.

What happens next, though, follows a by-now familiar logic: With only weak activation of the A- and H-detectors, only a moderate signal will be sent upward to the TH- and TA-detectors. Likewise, it seems plausible that only a moderate signal will be sent to the THE- and TAE-detectors at the word level. But, of course, the THE-detector is enormously well primed; if there is a TAE-detector. it would be barely primed, since this is a string that's rarely encountered. Thus, the THE- and TAE- detectors might be receiving similar input, but this input is sufficient only for the (well-primed) THE-detector, so only it will respond. In this way, the net will recognize the ambiguous pattern as "THE" not "TAE" (The same logic applies, of course, to the ambiguous pattern on the right, perceived as "CAT" not "CHT). A similar explanation will handle the word-superiority effect (see, e.g., Rumelhart & Siple, 1974). To take a simple case, imagine that present the letter A in the context "AT" If the presentation brief enough, participants may see very little of the A, perhaps just the horizontal crossbar. This wouldn't be enough to distinguish among A, F, or H, and so all these letter detectors would fire weakly. If this were all the information the participants had, they'd be stuck. But let's imagine that the participants did perceive the second letter the display, the T. It seems likely that the AT bigram is much better primed than the FT or HT bigrams. (That's because you often encounter words like "CAT" or "BOAT"; words like "SOFT" or "HEFT" are used less frequently.) Therefore, the weak firing of the A-detector would be enough to fire the AT bigram detector, while the weak firing for the F and H might not trigger their bigram detectors. In this way, a "choice" would be made at the bigram level that the input was "AT and not something else. Once this bigram has been detected, answering the question "Was there an A or an F in the display?" is easy. In this way, the letter will be better detected in context than in isolation. This isn't because context enables you to see more: instead, context allows you to make better use of what you see. Recognition Errors There is, however, a downside to all this. Imagine that we present the string "CQRN" to participants. If the presentation is brief enough, the participants will register only a subset of the string's features. Let's imagine that they register only the bottom bit of the string's second letter. This detection of the bottom curve will weakly activate the Q-detector and also the U-detector and the O-detector. The resulting pattern of network activation is shown in Figure 4.10. Of course, the pattern of activation here is exactly the same as it was in Figure 4.9. In both cases, perceivers have seen the features for the C, R, and N and have only seen the second letter's bottom curve. And we've already walked through the network's response to this feature pattern: This configuration will lead to confusion at the letter level, but the confusion will get sorted out at the bigram level, with the (primed) CO-detector responding to this input and other (less well primed) detectors not responding. As a result, the stimulus will be (mis)identified as "CORN." In the situation described in Figure 4.9, the stimulus actually was "CORN," and so the dynamic built into the net aids performance, allowing the network to recover from its initial confusion. In the case we're considering now (with "CQRN" as the stimulus), the exact same dynamic causes the network to misread the stimulus.

This example helps us understand how recognition errors occur and why those errors tend to make the input look more regular than it really is. The basic idea is that the network is biased favoring frequent letter combinations over infrequent ones. In effect, the network operates on the basis of "when in doubt, assume that the input falls into the frequent pattern." The reason, of course is simply that the detectors for the frequent pattern are well primed-and therefore easier to trigger.

Let's emphasize, though, that the bias built into the network facilitates perception if the input is, in fact, a frequent word, and these (by definition) are the words you encounter most of the time. The bias will pull the network toward errors if the input happens to have an unusual spelling pattern, but (by definition) these inputs are less common in your experience. Hence, the network's bias helps perception more often than it hurts. Distributed Knowledge We've now seen many indications that the network's functioning is guided by knowledge of spelling patterns. This is evident in the fact that letter strings are easier to recognize if they conform to normal spelling. The same point is evident in the fact that letter strings provide a context benefit (the WSE) only if they conform to normal spelling. Even more evidence comes from the fact that when errors occur, they "shift" the perception toward patterns of normal spelling.

To explain these results, we've suggested that the network "knows" (for example) that CO is a common bigram in English, while CF is not, and also "knows" that THE is a common sequence but TAE is not. The network seems to rely on this "knowledge" in "choosing its "interpretation" of unclear or ambiguous inputs. Similarly, the network seems to "expect" certain patterns and not others, and is more efficient when the input lines up with those "expectations." Obviously, we've wrapped quotations around several of these words to emphasize that the sense in which the net "knows" facts about spelling, or the sense in which it "expects" things or makes "interpretations," is a little peculiar. In reality, knowledge about spelling patterns isn't explicitly stored anywhere in the network. Nowhere within the net is there a sentence like "CO is a common bigram in English; CF is not." Instead, this memory (if we even want to call it that) is manifest only in the fact that the CO-detector happens to be more primed than the CF-detector. The CO-detector doesn't "know" anything about this advantage, nor does the CF-detector know anything about its disadvantage. Each one simply does its job, and in the course of doing their jobs, sometimes a "competition" will take place between these detectors. (This sort of competition was illustrated in Figures 4.9 and 4.10.) When these competitions occur, theyll be "decided" by activation levels: The better-primed detector will be more likely to respond and therefore will be more likely to influence subsequent events. That's the entire mechanism through which these "knowledge effects" arise. That's how "expectations" or "inferences" emerge-as a direct consequence of the activation levels.

To put this into technical terms, the network's "knowledge" is not locally represented anywhere; it isn't stored ina particular location or built into a specific process. As a result, we cannot look just at the level of priming in the CO-detector and conclude that this detector represents a frequently seen bigram. Nor can we look at the CF-detector and conclude that it represents a rarely seen bigram. Instead, we need to look at the relationship between these priming levels, and we also need to look at how this relationship will lead to one detector being more influential than the other. In this way, the knowledge about bigram frequencies is contained within the network via a distributed representation: it's knowledge, in other words, that's represented by a pattern of activations that's distributed across the network and detectable only if we consider how the entire network functions. What may be most remarkable about the feature net, then, lies in how much can be accomplished with a distributed representation, and thus with simple, mechanical elements correctly connected to one another. The net appears to make inferences and to know the rules of English spelling. But the actual mechanics of the net involve neither inferences nor knowledge (at least, not in any conventional sense). You and I can see how the inferences unfold by taking a bird's-eye view and considering how all the detectors work together as a system. But nothing in the net's functioning depends on the bird's-eye view. Instead, the activity of each detector is locally determined- influenced by just those detectors feeding into it. When all these detectors work together, though, no role in the result is a process that acts as if it knows the rules. But the rules themselves play guiding the network's moment-by-moment activities.

Efficiency versus Accuracy

One other point about the network needs emphasis: The network does make mistakes, misreading some inputs and misinterpreting some patterns. As we've seen, though, these errors are produced by exactly the same mechanisms that are responsible for the network's main advantages-its ability to deal with ambiguous inputs, for example, or to recover from confusion. Perhaps, therefore, we should view the errors as the price you pay in order to gain the benefits associated with the net: If you want a mechanism that's able to deal with unclear or partial inputs, you have to live with the fact that sometimes the mechanism will make errors.

But do you really need to pay this price? After all, outside of the lab you're unlikely to encounter fast-paced tachistoscopic inputs. Instead, you see stimuli that are out in view for long periods of time, stimuli that you can inspect at your leisure. Why, therefore, don't you take the moment to scrutinize these inputs so that you can rely on fewer inferences and assumptions, and in that way gain a higher level of accuracy in recognizing the objects you encounter? The answer is straightforward. To maximize accuracy, you could, in principle, scrutinize every misprinted, you would be sure to character on the page. That way, if a character were missing or detect it. But the cost associated with this strategy would be intolerable. Reading would be unspeakably slow (partly because the speed with which you move your eyes is relatively slow-no more than four or five eye movements per second). In contrast, it's possible to make inferences about a page with remarkable speed, and this leads readers to adopt the obvious strategy: They read some of the letters and make inferences about the rest. And for the most part, those inferences are safe-thanks to the simple fact that our language (like most aspects of our world) contains some redundncies, so that one doesn't need every lettr to identify what a wrd is; oftn the missng letter perfctly predctable from the contxt, virtually guaranteeing that inferences will be correct. e. Demonstration 4.3: Inferences in Reading The role of inferences within the normal process of reading is easy to demonstrate. In fact, you make these inferences even when you don't want to-that is, even when you're trying to read carefully. To see this, count how many times the letter F appears in the following passage. FINISHED FILES ARE THE RESULT OF YEARS OF SCIENTIFIC STUDYCOMBINED WITH THE EXPERIENCE OF YEARS. The correct answer is six; did you find them all? Many people miss one, two, or even three of the Fs. Why is this? In normal reading, you don't look at every letter, identifying it before you move on to the next. If you did, reading would be impossibly slow. (We can only move our eyes four or five times each second. If we looked at letter, we'd only be able to read about 5 characters per second, or 300 characters per minute. For most material, we read at a rate roughly 500% faster than this.)

Homework is Completed By:

Writer Writer Name Amount Client Comments & Rating
Instant Homework Helper

ONLINE

Instant Homework Helper

$36

She helped me in last minute in a very reasonable price. She is a lifesaver, I got A+ grade in my homework, I will surely hire her again for my next assignments, Thumbs Up!

Order & Get This Solution Within 3 Hours in $25/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 3 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

Order & Get This Solution Within 6 Hours in $20/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 6 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

Order & Get This Solution Within 12 Hours in $15/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 12 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

6 writers have sent their proposals to do this homework:

Instant Assignments
Buy Coursework Help
A+GRADE HELPER
Calculation Guru
Quality Homework Helper
Peter O.
Writer Writer Name Offer Chat
Instant Assignments

ONLINE

Instant Assignments

Hey, I can write about your given topic according to the provided requirements. I have a few more questions to ask as if there is any specific instructions or deadline issue. I have already completed more than 250 academic papers, articles, and technical articles. I can provide you samples. I believe my capabilities would be perfect for your project. I can finish this job within the necessary interval. I have four years of experience in this field. If you want to give me the project I had be very happy to discuss this further and get started for you as soon as possible.

$135 Chat With Writer
Buy Coursework Help

ONLINE

Buy Coursework Help

Hi dear, I am ready to do your homework in a reasonable price.

$142 Chat With Writer
A+GRADE HELPER

ONLINE

A+GRADE HELPER

Greetings! I’m very much interested to work on this project. I have read the details properly. I am a Professional Writer with over 5 years of experience, therefore, I can easily do this job. I will also provide you with TURNITIN PLAGIARISM REPORT. You can message me to discuss the detail. Why me? My goal is to offer services to you that are profitable. I don’t want you to place an order once and that’s it. For me to be successful, I need you to come back and order again. Give me the opportunity to work on your project. I wish to build a long-term relationship with you. We can have further discussion in chat. Thanks!

$135 Chat With Writer
Calculation Guru

ONLINE

Calculation Guru

I see that your standard of work is to get content for articles. Well, you are in the right place because I am a professional content writer holding a PhD. in English, as well as having immense experience in writing articles for a vast variety of niches and category such as newest trends, health issues, entertainment, technology, etc and I will make sure your article has all the key pointers and relevant information, Pros, Cons and basically all the information that a perfect article needs with good research. Your article is guaranteed to be appealing, attractive, engaging, original and passed through Copyscape for the audience so once they start reading they keep asking for more and stay interested.

$135 Chat With Writer
Quality Homework Helper

ONLINE

Quality Homework Helper

Hi dear, I am ready to do your homework in a reasonable price.

$142 Chat With Writer
Peter O.

ONLINE

Peter O.

Hello, I can assist you in writing attractive and compelling content on ganja and its movement globally. I will provide with valuable, informative content that you will appreciate. The content will surely hit your target audience. I will provide you with the work that will be according to the needs of the targeted audience and Google’s requirement.

$135 Chat With Writer

Let our expert academic writers to help you in achieving a+ grades in your homework, assignment, quiz or exam.

Similar Homework Questions

Freeman brown private school - Our fear of immigrants - Https assess shlonline com default action url - What are the elements of music - The crayon box that talked coloring page - Current carrying capacity table australia - Niko home control pdf - Which is a substantiated opinion - Management - Reading passage case study tourism new zealand website - The growth fund ekera dental - Calculate the molarities of the following solutions - Fnb premier cheque account fees - Color based object detection opencv python - Utah employer quarterly wage list - Andrea yates cognitive perspective - Mr bridge card tables - A heat pump is used to maintain a house - Cement stabilised sand strength - Cardiff university phd gown - Why does adenine pair with thymine - What does fed oasdi ee mean - Quantitative Evaluation 8381 - Discussion post - Concrete fire resistance rating - Glow stick liquid experiment - General Biology - Veneer wrapped mdf profiles https://studylib.net/doc/18873211/mdf-mouldings - Management information system of walmart - Universal ethical principles kohlberg - Job cover letter and resume - Where is lee on the hahn appliance commercials - Praxis source based essay sample - Discussion, Assignments - Cinema ___ film that is a homage to the cinema - 2212 ridge lane grapevine tx - Political cartoon dred scott decision - Joe kaiser deal or no deal episode 225 - Image of strigil and oil container found at pompeii - Beef cattle breeds ppt - Class c amplifier operation - Shall i wasting in despair paraphrase - What is fc physics - Case Study – Perioperative - 1911 idiot scratch prevention tool template - Kernel data structure in os - 5 8 full gas tank - What does personification do - Is have a simple predicate - Blackheath and bromley athletics - Help capsim comp xm - Measure of Sucess - MKGT201 - Nitin patel nuffield bristol - Transition communication plan template - Tristan Cottage - Marketing essentials assignment examples - Fuel cell fundamentals o hayre pdf - Enviromist industries pty ltd - Organ leader and decision making - Infotech in a global summary - DB 6 - Report on Landfills - What i am grateful for - Accounting class - Labs - Bsbmgt616 develop and implement strategic plans assessment 1 - Glen williams public school - DP 3 - Notetaking - Kiwi dna extraction lab - Nur495reply - Ego defensive function of attitude - Warnbro community high school - Database Optimization and Performance Tuning Paper - Geberit 65s pipe cutter - Dehydration of 2 methylcyclohexanol lab report - Kepnock state high school - Edward scissorhands characters personalities - Dynamic processes support operational activities - Family nursing research theory and practice friedman pdf - Healthcare Informatics and Technology - Assignment 2 PSY 7708 - Cds gross pollutant trap - Peoplesoft campus solutions ppt - Saving private ryan movie questions answers - Amu customer service - Molecular compound and covalent compound - As foolish as simile - Study of gods and goddesses - Newnan times herald arrest log - Left hydrocelectomy icd 10 pcs - Dorothy Orem theory - Discusssion - Intervention barriers - Cash disbursement journal entry - Question - Eight stones recruitment pte ltd - Ole miss management information systems - Law of attraction pick up line