Loading...

Messages

Proposals

Stuck in your homework and missing deadline? Get urgent help in $10/Page with 24 hours deadline

Get Urgent Writing Help In Your Essays, Assignments, Homeworks, Dissertation, Thesis Or Coursework & Achieve A+ Grades.

Privacy Guaranteed - 100% Plagiarism Free Writing - Free Turnitin Report - Professional And Experienced Writers - 24/7 Online Support

Describe the complexities of caesar's character

27/11/2021 Client: muhammad11 Deadline: 2 Day

Narrative, Vol 23, No. 2 (May 2015) Copyright 2015 by The Ohio State University

Miranda Anderson

Fission-Fusion Cognition in Shakespearean Drama: The Case for Julius Caesar 1

This paper examines how Renaissance notions of the mind and the subject, as con- strained and constituted by social means, are narrated and staged in Shakespeare’s Julius Caesar. This analysis is supplemented by a few references to Montaigne’s essays, whose influence on Shakespeare and concern with the nature of the mind and self are long established (Ellrodt). To further ground the case, it begins with two brief over- views: firstly, on narratological approaches to drama and their particular relevance to Renaissance drama, and secondly, on various current approaches to social cognition. I focus on the linked concepts that a multiplicity of agents can operate within a single human being, and conversely that multiple individuals can form a cognitive unit. These related notions of the mind as social, both in Renaissance fictional and factual narratives and in current cognitive science, are understood to be due to human psy- chophysiological capacities. These capacities both afford and require boundaries and flow between the constituent parts of the self, both as regards those within skull or skin, and as regards those in the world. As I want to highlight the issue of divisions, as well as sharing, between individuals and within an individual I have adopted the

Miranda Anderson is the initiator of, and a research fellow on, the History of Distributed Cognition Proj- ect, based at the University of Edinburgh. This project expands on research completed during her Lever- hulme Early Career Fellowship, which explores parallels (and contrasts) between recent philosophical the- ories on the extended mind and analogous ideas in literary, philosophical, and scientific texts circulating between the fifteenth and early-seventeenth century. A number of papers on this research are forthcoming along with her book, The renaissance extended Mind (London: Palgrave Macmillan, 2015). She can be reached at miranda.anderson@ed.ac.uk.

Fission-Fusion Cognition in Julius Caesar 155

physics term “fission-fusion,” which has been used by ethology to describe dynamic social networks that periodically merge and divide, and I have reapplied it specifically to cognition in order to capture the malleable and shifting nature of the cognitive units formed (Aureli et al.).

Dramatizing Narrative

Piece out our imperfections with your thoughts . . . For ’tis your thoughts that now must deck our kings . . . (Henry v 1.1.23, 1.1.28)

This famous prologue might initially seem a more likely candidate than the prologue- less Julius Caesar for a defense of a narratological reading of the social mind in Renais- sance drama. Firstly, because the appeal to the audience implies their participation is necessary for an effective creation of the playworld. Secondly, because prologues have been interpreted as providing a structural equivalent to the narrator’s role in prose fic- tion, on which classical definitions of narrative have traditionally drawn. Yet research on Renaissance English playscripts from around 1600 suggests that such narratolog- ically-attuned framing features as prologues, choruses, and epilogues, were not nec- essarily permanent, but were often created for first or for special performances only, with many lost and a few relocated from one play to another (Stern). They are tempo- rary, absent, or unreliable features. This capacity of a play to dispense with a single ex- plicit privileged narrator and to instead leave only a polyphonic array of characters is in itself suggestive of the relevance of drama to exploring notions of the social mind; this capacity also obliquely indicates the relevance of more recent notions of narratol- ogy that are not dependent on structures derived from a narrator’s roles.

Monika Fludernik bases drama’s inclusion in narratological studies on its abil- ity to evoke experientiality and a fictional world, with the minimum requirement the presence of a character on stage (“Narrative and Drama”). Fludernik further de- scribes the way the developing novel adopted the “deep-structural patterns of drama” (“Diachronization” 343). In turn, as Barbara Hardy relates, Shakespeare makes nar- rative theatrical, for as Hardy argues: “[n]arrative is a primary act of mind, a way of comprehending and constructing social and psychic life, as inevitable and central a subject of drama as of prose fiction” (24). Furthermore, there is an additional level of richness to theatrical performance’s treatment of narrative, as Nünning and Sommer have described:

plays do not just represent narratives (i.e. a series of events), they also stage narratives in that, more often than not, they make storytelling, i.e. the art of telling narratives, theatrical. In other words, plays not only represent series of events [mimetic narrativity] they also represent ‘acts of narration,’ with characters serving as intradiegetic storytellers. (337)

Therefore, rather than attempting to give an account of narrative in Shakespeare that

156 Miranda anderson

attempts to comply with classical narrative notions, this paper will explore the ways in which Shakespeare himself reflexively employed narrative in one of his works.

Regarding the anxiety to please that marks such entreaties as Puck’s promise, “[i]f you pardon, we will mend,” Tiffany Stern comments that not only these stage- orations but also the main script is open to revision; this possibility of revision em- phasizes the potential impact an audience may have on a performance and even on a playscript (119). The presumed priority and dominance of playscript over perfor- mance is another feature discussed in narratological considerations of drama (for example, see Jahn 692); this dominance does not hold true for the Renaissance, and again suggests the social nature of the mind brought to bear on the playscript. An- other feature considered in narrative accounts of drama are the stage directions that guide an actual performance or a reader’s virtualization of it (Schaeffer 257). Yet in Shakespeare the narratorial hand tends to operate through implicit instructions and narrative details embedded in the dialogue; these often combine metaphorical im- ages with a demand for stage action, such as Ligarius’s statement “I here discard my sickness” (Shakespeare 2.1.320), which both symbolises his rejection of bondage and requires his removal of a dressing (see Pasternak Slater 144 and Tribble). Fludernik describes fiction and drama as differing primarily on the discourse level, with drama offering the setting’s visual presentation instead of a verbal description (“Narrative and Drama” 361–62); this is less the case, though, with Shakespeare’s works, where background descriptions are often given as a verbal supplement to the relatively bare stage. In general, then, what would commonly constitute didascalia is embedded intratextually, and this indicates that Shakespearean drama is not quite as open to “pure invention” as has generally been imagined (Claycomb 173), although it lacks what Manfred Jahn calls the “controlling ‘frame’” of lengthy stage directions (672).

Social Mind

In his study of ship navigation, Ed Hutchins makes a case for cognitive systems as distributed through equipment, which incorporates aspects of necessary expertise, as well as through other social agents, like the ship’s navigational team that operates collectively as a computational system (155; xiv). The seminal paper on “The Ex- tended Mind” by Andy Clark and David Chalmers mentions that one of the ways in which cognitive processes extend into the world is through other people (17). Deb Tollefsen elaborates on this with the hypothetical example of a forgetful philosopher and his wife Inga, who enables him to orient himself in the world. Stephen Kosslyn names this notion “social prosthetic systems” (SPSs): systems in which other social agents act as mind-extensions. SPSs are other people whom we “rely on . . . to ex- tend our reasoning abilities and to help us regulate and constructively employ our emotions,” so that “other people’s brains come to serve as extensions of your own brain” (“What Do You Believe”). There is a distinction here, in that Hutchins’s ac- counts focus on collaborative group minds, rather than the socially extended minds that assign generative and receptive functions to one individual or another, which

Fission-Fusion Cognition in Julius Caesar 157

are offered by Clark, Chalmers, Tollefsen, and Kosslyn: each account raises different issues of agency. Yet all of these accounts highlight the benefits of social extended- ness, whereas psychoanalytic and postmodern accounts tend to expose the prob- lematic elements while eliding the role of the body and the non-sociocultural envi- ronment. Both positive and negative aspects of social extendedness, and its relation to being embodied and embedded, are explored in this analysis of Renaissance texts, which themselves explore these concerns.

The mirror neuron system has been credited with enabling socially extended cognition, since when one person watches another performing actions, experienc- ing emotions or narrating action words mirror neurons fire in the motor cortex as if the watcher were performing the actions and experiencing the emotions themselves (Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia). Commenting on the theatrical insights provided by the discovery of the mirror neuron system, Bruce McConachie reflects that “[o]ur abil- ity to empathize with the experiences of others though mirroring is the cognitive hook that impels spectator interest in the activities of actor/characters and engages us in the unfolding narrative of a play” (18). Yet studies have shown that individuals with specialized experience of the action performed have a greater intensity of firing in the mirror neuron system than do non-experts (Calvo-Merino). So while mirror neurons indicate the potential for considerable sharing of experience across persons, this evidence suggests that there is also considerable particularity, due to the contin- gency of subjective experience (Anderson et al., “Involving Interface” 322). Theater is a medium with an immediate feedback loop between actors and audience, and yet since the mental states do not remain identical across different individuals, with the intensity of the mirror neuron firing affected by our own cognitive repertoire, it makes sense that one’s subjective experiences of theater, as with those in the world, involve consistencies and divergences with those of others. We experience fusions and fissions with a dramatic spectacle and with the reactions of others in an audience.

The human capacity to form ad hoc fusions as a means to undertake cognitive processing is underwritten by internal fissions. One’s subjective experience is also dependent on a number of contingencies, so that an individual’s experience of a play on one day or the next will also not be identical. A number of theories argue that distinct phenomenological experience is accompanied by internal fissions. Thomas Nagel argues that while third-person accounts leave out the subjective nature of con- sciousness, our “simple idea of a single person will come to seem quaint some day, when the complexities of the human control system become clearer and we become less certain that there is anything very important that we are one of ” (164). While the function of consciousness is social, Jakob Hohwy posits, it is for this very reason that it remains private, as it thereby enables us to optimally integrate other people’s vari- ant perceptual estimates of the world (254). Daniel Dennett instead proposes that the habit of adopting an “intentional stance,” that is, viewing behavior in terms of men- tal properties, spreads “to cover both other-interpretation and self-interpretation” (159); this implies a shared mechanism for making inferences about our own and others’ mental experiences. Dennett’s notion that it is intersubjectivity that is prima- ry and that precedes first-person subjectivity is echoed by both Alan Palmer and Da- vid Herman (Palmer, Fictional Minds 5, 178; Herman 762). Herman further argues

158 Miranda anderson

against the pervasive literary theory of anti-intentionalism, which claims that it is an error to attempt to assess the intention of an author from his work. Herman points out that anti-intentionalism is counterintuitive to humans’ propensity to ascribe in- tentions to others, including authors and characters within works, instead he argues that this is as necessary and helpful a part of our readings of literature, as it is of peo- ple in real life. Furthermore, within literary texts this ascription of intentions is play- fully explored: it is self-consciously staged by Shakespeare in his works, which both depict and rely on humans’ propensity to observe others’ behavior, actions and words and thereby to ascribe attitudes and beliefs to them (both correctly and wrongly).

Charles Fernyhough highlights the social nature of many aspects of internal cognition. Yet this, he argues, places in question Alan Palmer’s emphasis on the dis- tinction between intermental and intramental thought, although Palmer himself in fact argues that they form a “continuum” (Social Minds 39). Fernyhough conceives of cognition in rather narrow postmodernist terms, calling it “fundamentally semi- otic” (272–3). Were this view held valid, it would more markedly undermine Palmer’s achievement in shifting the focus on cognition in the novel from purely verbal forms to a wider spectrum, but Palmer’s broader view of cognition is supported both by his own works and by recent cognitive scientific research on the nontrivial roles that em- bodiment and environment play in cognitive processes. Palmer’s claim that mental states are not only conveyed through equivalent forms to speech acts in the novel is also pertinent here, since Shakespeare’s narratives, as well as being potentially enacted on a stage, contain embodied and experiential descriptions as a means to convey cog- nitive processes in action.

The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy’s article on collective intentionality, which is defined as “the power of minds to be jointly directed at objects, matters of fact, states of affairs, goals, or values” acknowledges that “this part of the history of philosophy is largely unwritten,” as it skips from Aristotle to Rousseau and German Idealism in a few sentences, before going on to discuss movements from the late 19th century onwards in more detail (Schweikard and Schmid). This may have led to the type of risky claim made by Palmer that the debate on to “what extent is it possible to have knowledge of the workings of other minds?” and to “what purposes should our knowledge of other minds be put?” began in the early nineteenth century (Social Minds 5). Evidence for such a debate on this enduring human trait in Julius Caesar is explored in the next section, and the further question of to what extent internal cog- nition also involves ad hoc coalitions of quasi-autonomous agencies that require first- person inference is touched on. Finally, another debate newly raised here concerns the extent to which discourse between two characters constitutes a cognitive process (for further Renaissance instances see Anderson, The renaissance extended Mind).

Julius Caesar

The co-authoring of plays or re-use of source material results in different forms of creative collaboration than those discussed above. The story of Shakespeare’s Julius

Fission-Fusion Cognition in Julius Caesar 159

Caesar is primarily based on Thomas North’s translation of Plutarch’s Lives of the Noble Greeks and romans. A multiperspectival account of the murder scene progres- sively accumulates details across Plutarch’s lives of Julius Caesar, Brutus, and Antony in a way that Shakespeare mirrors in his play. In Shakespeare the sudden onslaught of Caesar’s stabbing is staged and reported in metaleptical, proleptical, and analepti- cal narratives, which provoke a consideration of the respective merits of mimetic and diegetic media and of the power of their combined or independent use. Yet the nar- ratives that evoke the workings of minds are more reliant on notions we also find ex- pressed in Montaigne. Montaigne himself admired Plutarch, quoting him nearly 400 times in the essays, but the nature of the mind is little discussed in the relevant lives, except a brief but pertinent passage which depicts Cassius explaining to Brutus the senses, the humors, and the imagination’s pliability and unreliability (136). There are also a few brief conventional mentions of the conspirators as being “all of one mind,” of Cassius as “compelled against my mind and will” to wage all on the unsuccessful battle, and of “Cassius’ friends which were of his mind before” regarding the battle becoming “of Brutus’ mind” (118, 139). These helpfully gesture towards the everyday nature of notions of a fused group mind, cognitive fission, and socially extended cog- nition, but do not explore the ramifications as does Shakespeare.

In a typically Shakespearean sleight of hand, Julius Caesar’s assassination by the conspirators is immediately followed by a metadramatic anticipation of its later the- atrical performance. In Genette’s terms, albeit reapplied to drama, this plays on the “temporal duality” (Narrative Discourse 33) of the event in the world and in the play:

Cassius How many ages hence Shall this our lofty scene be acted over In states unborn and accents yet unknown! Brutus How many times shall Caesar bleed in sport, That now on Pompey’s basis lies along No worthier than the dust! Cassius So oft as that shall be, So often shall the knot of us be called The men that gave their country liberty. (3.1.111–18)

This simultaneously highlights the illusory and the historically real nature of the scene, and yet rather than creating fission between actors and audience, it is creative of a conspiratorial intimacy. The characters are moved into a position of external focalization on their actions that more closely approximates and frames the audi- ence’s reaction. It amplifies the audience’s involvement, as conversely the audience is seemingly sucked back in time out of their world to become witnesses to the after- math of the original murder. This shared narrative, which imagines a future staging, indirectly appears to verify the reality of the current enactment, magnifying a sense of triumph.

This is juxtaposed with Antony’s narrative, which foresees not future applause but the destructive sequence of events that will follow, and which grants a horrifying narrative agency to the speechless corpse:

160 Miranda anderson

Over thy wounds now do I prophesy, (Which, like dumb mouths, do ope their ruby lips, To beg the voice and utterance of my tongue) A curse shall light upon the limbs of men; Domestic fury and fierce civil strife Shall cumber all the parts of Italy (3.1.259–64)

The capacity of the gory gaping body itself to speak and direct events, to spout blood flow through the ensuing scenes, is brought about through Antony’s giving voice and agency to it. Antony undercuts the self-aggrandizing narrative of the faction though the fusion of his identity with Caesar’s corpse, so enabling Caesar’s power to reach be- yond his death. In describing the motivation for human co-operation in social pros- thetic systems, Kosslyn writes that “if one perceives that one’s identity is distributed over the group, one will not perceive one’s death as obliterating the self—and to some extent, this perception is not erroneous” (“Evolution” 551). In such ways Caesar mo- tivates events even posthumously (as Pompey’s statue looming over Caesar’s corpse also seems to achieve a posthumous revenge).

Shakespeare simultaneously explores and exposes the combined techniques of showing and telling through which he himself moves the audience. He plays on the concerns voiced by anti-theatricalists about playgoing, and by the elite about the un- derclasses, with both underprivileged groundlings and the nobility in the galleries making up his audience; the first, he mocks through his representation of the feigning Cassius who “loves no plays” (1.2.202), and the second, he prods through the many- voiced but seemingly single-minded “Plebeians.” Evoking both concerns, the tribune Flavius in the play’s opening lines comically commands: “Hence! home, you idle crea- tures, get you home! / Is this a holiday?” (1.1.1–2). The plebeians’ easily swayed group mind is first placated by Brutus’s account of the need for Caesar’s murder, then gradu- ally roused back again to uproar by Antony:

If you have tears, prepare to shed them now. You all do know this mantle: I remember The first time ever Caesar put it on; ’Twas on a summer’s evening, in his tent, That day he overcame the Nervii: Look, in this place ran Cassius’ dagger through: See what a rent the envious Casca made: Through this the well-beloved Brutus stabbed; . . . For when the noble Caesar saw him stab, Ingratitude, more strong than traitor’s arms, Quite vanquished him: then burst his mighty heart; And, in his mantle muffling up his face, Even at the base of Pompey’s statua, Which all the while ran blood, great Caesar fell. O, what a fall was there, my countrymen! Then I, and you, and all of us fell down, Whilst bloody treason flourished over us. (3.2.167–74, 3.2.182–90)

Fission-Fusion Cognition in Julius Caesar 161

Antony generates the crowd’s pity and fury by pointedly juxtaposing one of Caesar’s martial triumphs with his violent stabbing by his supposed friends and with the beloved Brutus striking the killing blow. Giovanna Colombetti discusses how the sharing of affective states across groups creates social bonding and collective action; and here it later leads to reckless rampage and the absurd murder of Cinna the poet only for sharing the name of Cinna the conspirator (194–202; 3.3). Antony further amplifies their empathy, by depicting the fall as not of Caesar alone, but of them all jointly at the conspirators’ hands, as physical and figural are superimposed. In this further reverberation of the assassination and of its earlier replay, Antony places the crowd at the scene in the same way the audience in the theater have been placed by Cassius’s and Brutus’s speeches, but here the crowd become the falling Caesar. This further reverberation also invites the audience’s critical self-reflexivity regarding their own cognitive and affective susceptibility and pliability. There is a doubling of engagement through the supplementation of the visual by the verbal and the verbal by the visual, with a climax the uncovering of the still bleeding body. Antony evokes immediacy and bloodiness in a graphically forceful and materially mediated way, so overturning Brutus’s spare and restrained rhetoric that appealed to rationality, virtue and honor as humanity’s motivating forces. He simultaneously claims for his account a transparency that he contrasts with Brutus’s rhetoric, further heightening the power of his physically figurative and anti-rhetorical rhetoric over the crowd.

Another angle on the difference is offered by Schaeffer, who distinguishes be- tween forms of fiction in terms of “immersion vectors” (the means by which we access a fictional universe) and related “immersion postures” (the perspectives as- signed to us by the vectors): these occur along an axis that stretches from “purely mental immersion (the one that is induced by a fictional tale) to immersion in an inner-worldly situation (the one of a theatre actor . . . )” (218–19). Schaeffer describes how the “simulation of inner-worldly elements” occurs in a theater, as the two uni- verses of reality and fiction superimpose on each other: it uses real physical space, objects and human beings and therefore assigns to the spectator not the simulation of a perspective flux (as in the cinema), but a vector of “attending to events” (224). Yet Shakespeare’s theatrical works often simultaneously deploy a range of immersion vectors or set them against one another; for example, this scene elicits the human capacities for mental immersion through attending to a tale and through attending to onstage events.

The narrative that sets this all in motion is Cassius’s claim to Brutus that he can act as mirror to his mind. Brutus’s third person account of himself to Cassius as in- ternally fissured, “poor Brutus, with himself at war,” prompts Cassius’s cynical inter- vention. In a passage absent in Plutarch’s Lives, Cassius, who is intent on persuading Caesar’s protégé Brutus to participate in Caesar’s assassination, attempts to convince him of the necessity of socially extended reflexivity:

Cassius Tell me, good Brutus, can you see your face? Brutus No, Cassius, for the eye sees not itself But by reflection, by some other things. Cassius ’Tis just; And it is very much lamented, Brutus,

162 Miranda anderson

That you have no such mirrors as will turn Your hidden worthiness into your eye, That you might see your shadow . . . And since you know you cannot see yourself So well as by reflection, I, your glass, Will modestly discover to yourself That of yourself which you yet know not of. (1.2.50–60, 1.2.69–72)

Cassius attempts to convince Brutus that the limits of perception, the inability of the face and the eyes to see themselves other than through the process of reflection, are akin to the limits of introspection, of a parallel psychological inability of the sub- ject to apprehend its own qualities, without a form of socially extended reflexivity. Montaigne comments that others can often draw more from him than he can find in himself, and that sometimes when he has lost the point of what he was saying, that a stranger may discover it before he could (31–32). As in Dennett’s more extreme stance the mind is potentially more transparent to another than to oneself; and as in Kosslyn’s theory, other people’s minds are described as acting as cognitive supple- ments to one’s biological capacities. The significance of this offering of extended re- flexivity can best be understood through attention to the distinctively human capacity of reflective knowledge in Renaissance schemas, since there is an understanding that what marks out the human mind is its ability to reflect on itself: “The difference be- tween the Reason of man, and the Instinct of the beast is this, That the beast does but know, but the man knows that he knows” (Donne 8, 11.225).

But here Cassius proposes the external medium of his extrospective perspective instead as the necessary supplement to Brutus’s troubled introspection; this also sug- gests the way in which language utilizes and extends our capacity to be both receptive to and generative of cognition in others. Recast in Tollefsen’s terms, Cassius offers to act as an Inga for Brutus. Yet Shakespeare here explores the problematics of such cog- nitive social supplements. The self-interest involved in Cassius’s claim to be the bearer of Brutus’s true reflection is revealed in an aside: “If I were Brutus now, and he were Cassius, / He should not humour me” (1.2.313–14). This counterfactual exchange of positions counterpoints his explicit mirroring of Brutus. Cassius’s sense of superior- ity, despite his inferior social status, is due to his capacity to manipulate Brutus while remaining seemingly untouched and opaque. Yet again in the aside, support is given to the underlying notion of socially constituted cognitive processes and subjectivity: “Thy honourable mettle may be wrought / From that it is disposed. Therefore it is meet / That noble minds keep ever with their likes” (1.2.307–10). By the end, though, it is evident that Machiavellian minds are not immune to cognitive contagion either. Yet Cassius’s extrospective perspective here is shown to be problematically motivated by his own opaque subjective aims. The notion of cognitive reception and generation as potentially involving only a partial fusion is echoed in the primarily one-way ma- nipulation of the masses by Brutus and Antony. This latter aspect of social cognition can also be framed in terms of Karin Kukkonen’s theory of “top-heavy” social minds (“Top-Heavy Social Minds”).

The notion of cognitive and moral susceptibility ties in both with anti-theatrical- ists’ and pro-theatricalists’ beliefs about the capacity of theater: while Thomas White

Fission-Fusion Cognition in Julius Caesar 163

asserted that “the cause of plagues is sinne, if you looke to it well: and the cause of sinne are playes: therefore the cause of plagues are playes” (47), Thomas Heywood describes how theater “hath power to new mold the harts of the spectators and fash- ion them to the shape of any noble and notable attempt” (sig. B4r). Either way, theater is understood as an emulatory activity that morally alters and cognitively shapes the spectator. Shakespeare self-consciously stages the human tendency to observe others’ behavior, actions, and words and to ascribe attitudes and beliefs to them; this staging bears out the relevance of Herman’s argument about the general human propensity to employ intentionality and for us as readers or spectators to apply it in our analysis of Shakespeare. From another angle this reflects the problem with fusion indicated by Jakob Hohwy: by not simply merging another’s view with one’s own, by maintaining fission and resisting fusion, one retains a range of available interpretations; whereas to the extent one submits to fusion with another mind, one may be cognitively and morally altered for the worse. Conversely, though, such fusions may be for the better. In fact, it is Cassius who is morally and cognitively altered, for although he remains flawed, he ends by offering a noble albeit doomed verbal mirroring of Brutus. Rather than the anti-theatricalists, then, we might argue for a theatrical staging of the posi- tive pro-theatricalist narrative with a touching (though not untroubled) reciprocity in the final scenes, in contradistinction to the earlier proffered generative, one-way, extended reflexivity.

Brutus is further roused to action through letters apparently authored by citi- zens but which the audience know are sent by Cassius, which echo and seem to vali- date Cassius’s plea, calling on Brutus to awake and “see thyself;” this fission between audience and character perspectives heightens the pathos of Brutus’s credulity (2.1; 1.2.314–16). Brutus attempts yet fails to create introspectively a state of revelatory self-reflection. That Brutus’s internal fissures have been intensified by Cassius’s pro- ferred reflexivity, he describes in a soliloquy in which he attempts to reason it out:

Since Cassius first did whet me against Caesar, I have not slept. Between the acting of a dreadful thing And the first motion, all the interim is Like a phantasma, or a hideous dream: The Genius and the mortal instruments Are then in council; and the state of man, Like to a little kingdom, suffers then The nature of an insurrection. (2.1.61–69)

Brutus’s internal prefiguring of civil uprising reflects the conventional understanding of the quasi-autonomous agencies that constitute the self, and the analogy between subject and world. Brutus here adopts a Dennettian “intentional stance” as a means to fathom his own mental unrest; however, it finally does not generate insights so much as a justificatory narrative.

Montaigne similarly describes the conflict created by his many-faceted mutabil- ity and the inferential stance consequently required, which he argues reflects a more general human predicament:

164 Miranda anderson

I give my soul now one face, now another, according to which direction I turn it. If I speak of myself in different ways, that is because I look at myself in different ways . . . All contradictions may be found in me by some twist and in some fashion . . . and whoever studies himself really attentively finds in himself, yes, even in his judgment, this gyration and discord. (293–94)

As in Brutus’s case, consciousness and self-consciousness are both problematically implicated, with this fissured experientiality iconically staged in the twists and turns of the multi-claused sentence structure. Both fictional and non-fictional narratives can convey the transparency and opacity of others (and oneself ). Like Hohwy, Mon- taigne also argues that to minimize the tendency to make errors one must maintain distance from others’ viewpoints, though this is necessarily limited from the start by the extent to which the social is already within: it “is not enough to get away from the crowd; we must get away from the gregarious instincts that are inside us” in an attempt to “sequester and repossess ourselves” (213). Montaigne describes how we imagine as morally right what are actually inherited notions, as we confuse local cus- toms with the natural laws of conscience and reason (100). The extended or collective mind is no more reliable than that of an individual.

Despite their collective intention to assassinate Caesar, with the basis for this a diachronic form of group mind borne out of their shared Roman blood and ancestors, fissions are also evident in the faction: in a farcical prefiguring of the misperceptions of the fatal battle, they cannot even agree which way (the literal and symbolic) dawn lies (2.1.100–110). Similarly, despite their joint aim and sworn allegiance, Antony and Octavius later cannot agree who will attack from the left or right (5.1.16–20). Cas- sius’s transformation is also preceded by an internal fission, caused by Brutus’s moral upbraiding, though it is Brutus’s later confession of displaying ill temper himself that leads to reconciliation, alongside a misogynist (but conventional) casting off of Cas- sius’s outburst as generated by his mother: “When you are over-earnest with your Brutus, / He’ll think your mother chides, and leave you so” (4.3.121–22). Thus a rel- egation of an internal aspect of Cassius’s character to an external agency allows for a renewed fusion of hands and hearts between Cassius and Brutus.

Cassius’s attempts to lead Brutus ultimately fail, as Brutus idealistically persuades him not to kill Antony, by describing Caesar and Antony as if they are one agent (composed of unequal parts) and claiming that “Our course will seem too bloody . . . / To cut the head off and then hack the limbs” (2.1.161–62). Cassius in the spirit of reconciliation is persuaded to set all on one ill-advised battle against his own will. It is, then, the choleric and cynical Cassius who is finally transformed from his much-cited statement of Epicurean self-determination, “The fault, dear Brutus, is not in our stars, / But in ourselves, that we are underlings,” to the less-cited admission of the wider sphere of influences by which we are constituted: “Now I change my mind, / And partly credit things that do presage” (1.2.141–42, 5.1.77–78). As Werner Wolf and Brian Richardson have argued, narratives provide sense-making and world-modeling functions, with the representation of chance and causation providing privileged ac- cess to implied worldviews.

Fission-Fusion Cognition in Julius Caesar 165

By the tragic end Cassius mirrors Brutus’s words in conformity not in coercion:

Brutus For ever, and for ever, farewell, Cassius. If we do meet again, why, we shall smile. If not, why then, this parting was well made. Cassius For ever, and for ever, farewell, Brutus. If we do meet again, we’ll smile indeed. If not, ’tis true this parting was well made. (5.3.117–22)

The nostalgic mirroring the narrative creates an electrifying intensity and yet hypnot- ic lulling through the uncanny mimicry of the narrative’s reiteration. This reiteration transmits an echoing sensation, and a sense of looking forward and backward simul- taneously via the counterfactual imagined futures. Against his original Machiavellian intentions Cassius ironically becomes the true mirror he claimed to be to Brutus.

Homework is Completed By:

Writer Writer Name Amount Client Comments & Rating
Instant Homework Helper

ONLINE

Instant Homework Helper

$36

She helped me in last minute in a very reasonable price. She is a lifesaver, I got A+ grade in my homework, I will surely hire her again for my next assignments, Thumbs Up!

Order & Get This Solution Within 3 Hours in $25/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 3 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

Order & Get This Solution Within 6 Hours in $20/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 6 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

Order & Get This Solution Within 12 Hours in $15/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 12 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

6 writers have sent their proposals to do this homework:

Unique Academic Solutions
Top Academic Tutor
24/7 Assignment Help
Instant Assignments
Premium Solutions
Top Quality Assignments
Writer Writer Name Offer Chat
Unique Academic Solutions

ONLINE

Unique Academic Solutions

I am an experienced researcher here with master education. After reading your posting, I feel, you need an expert research writer to complete your project.Thank You

$41 Chat With Writer
Top Academic Tutor

ONLINE

Top Academic Tutor

I am an academic and research writer with having an MBA degree in business and finance. I have written many business reports on several topics and am well aware of all academic referencing styles.

$48 Chat With Writer
24/7 Assignment Help

ONLINE

24/7 Assignment Help

I have worked on wide variety of research papers including; Analytical research paper, Argumentative research paper, Interpretative research, experimental research etc.

$18 Chat With Writer
Instant Assignments

ONLINE

Instant Assignments

I reckon that I can perfectly carry this project for you! I am a research writer and have been writing academic papers, business reports, plans, literature review, reports and others for the past 1 decade.

$46 Chat With Writer
Premium Solutions

ONLINE

Premium Solutions

I am an experienced researcher here with master education. After reading your posting, I feel, you need an expert research writer to complete your project.Thank You

$37 Chat With Writer
Top Quality Assignments

ONLINE

Top Quality Assignments

I find your project quite stimulating and related to my profession. I can surely contribute you with your project.

$32 Chat With Writer

Let our expert academic writers to help you in achieving a+ grades in your homework, assignment, quiz or exam.

Similar Homework Questions

SDLC Program & Programming - 633.d2.1 - Module 5 Case Study: Flow and Pull - Mathsmadeeasy a level biology - Physics a level syllabus - High hurlands care home - What is the unknown metal identified as in this lab - Garth owns two all terrain vehicles - Module 10 Post-Lab Quiz - What are stakeholders in sport - Thrt c - The task of an organization is reflected in its - Alibaba case study solution - Dartford tag top up - Vocabulario mas usado en ingles - Shake it to morella - Nursing - Systems understanding aid solutions 9th edition - Raf kinloss station commanders - The product spanx is an example of an ordinary innovation - Reflection Course (Due 24 Hours) - Discussion question - Assessment due in 48 hours - Virgin media leased line - System Design - Paper writing - Volume to Value: Executive Brief - What approaches could have yielded additional valuable information - St martin guide to writing 8th edition pdf - Synchronous vs asynchronous transmission - Maths,physics and chemistry - Abductor digiti minimi stretch - Atlas god roman name - It 210 final project milestone 1 - A conversational textbook on technical writing brian quinn - Methyl benzoate by fischer esterification - Pka of indicators list - 4 Responses Sep 03 - Computer modern font mac - Patchless ride for autism 2017 - Discussion - Stance essay - English sba reflection 2 outline - Kaydon ring and seal - Introduction to java programming comprehensive version 11e pdf - Reading question - Visual reflection essay format - Snitches end up in ditches - The last dance encountering death and dying 10th edition - Siemens s110 servo drive manual - Does bass pro take apple pay - Coach greg cookbook 2.0 pdf - Harry potter description extract - Sigma aldrich msds sheets - A lack of cooperation by oligopolists trying to maintain monopoly profits - Directly proportional to the square - Professional development for nurse practitioners - Easy cite rmit - Example of macrosystem in healthcare - Market entry modes for international businesses chapter 7 - Average weather over a long period of time - Family Business 5 - Descriptive Memoir Essay - Discussion - Simon's personality in lord of the flies - What is unfired pressure vessel - Marketing - Educate or train leaders army essay blc - A pair of tickets short story summary - Parallel boost graph library - I need help with creating a Marketing Supply Chain - SPSS - Imac 2014 wall mount - Mickey lawson is considering investing some money - He shall spurn fate scorn death - A surgeon is using material from a donated heart - Essay - Adult power point - How to apply total cell style in excel - Who was eleanor rigby - Cain and abel's offering - Science lab rules poster - Mornington peninsula walking tracks - The standard unmodified audit report for nonpublic companies - What is required for cm3 - Mla format informative speech outline - She did deceive her father marrying you - Cowards cakes mildura west - #7 - Preparing for the future of work. - Gentex commander 3 fire alarm - Types of probability ppt - Miss clairol helena maria viramontes pdf - Lauren lake's paternity court myers vs black - Dehydration of 2 methylcyclohexanol lab report - Discuss Question - Curtin oasis blackboard login - Assignment 2.2: Liberty Challenged in Nineteenth Century America Final Paper - Bible revelation chapter 12 - The globalization of eating disorders rhetorical analysis