Toilet Training Article Review
ou are to write a minimum of a 3-5 page double spaced, 12 point font (minimum) summary of the following article:
Fox & Azrin (1971) Click for more options Fox & Azrin (1971) - Alternative Formats
Address each of the following subheadings:
Problem
Procedure
Results
Discussion
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS
A RAPID METHOD OF TOILET TRAINING THE INSTITUTIONALIZED RETARDED'
N. H. AZRIN AND R. M. Foxx
ANNA STATE HOSPITAL AND SOUTHERN ILLINOIS UNIVERSITY
Incontinence is a major unsolved problem in the institutional care of the profoundly retarded. A reinforcement and social analysis of incontinence was used to develop a procedure that would rapidly toilet train retardates and motivate them to remain con- tinent during the day in their ward setting. Nine profoundly retarded adults were given intensive training (median of four days per patient), the distinctive features of which were artificially increasing the frequency of urinations, positive reinforcement of correct toileting but a delay for "accidents", use of new automatic apparatus for signalling elim- ination, shaping of independent toileting, cleanliness training, and staff reinforcement procedures. Incontinence was reduced immediately by about 90% and eventually decreased to near-zero. These results indicate the present procedure is an effective, rapid, enduring, and administratively feasible solution to the problem of incontinence of the institu- tionalized retarded.
Institutionalized patients, especially the pro- foundly retarded, often urinate and defecate in their clothing during the day. Several stud- ies (Azrin, Bugle, and O'Brien, in press; Baumeister and Klosowski, 1965; Bensberg, Colwell, and Cassell, 1965; Dayan, 1964; Giles and Wolf, 1966; Hundziak, Maurer, and Wat- son, 1965; Kimbrell, Luckey, Barbuto, and Love, 1967; Van Wagenen, Meyerson, Kerr, and Mahoney, 1969) have reported some de- gree of success in reducing this daytime incon- tinence with institutionalized retardates or mental patients (Wagner and Paul, 1970) by using reinforcement principles. Several of these efforts have been primarily concerned with "habit training", i.e., successful elimi- nation in the toilet bowl when the retardate is placed there at regularly and frequently scheduled intervals (Dayan, 1964; Baumeister
1This investigation was supported by the Illinois De- partment of Mental Health and Grant 17981 from the National Institute of Mental Health. Grateful acknowl- edgment of assistance is given to C. Bugle, J. Crider, F. Gould, D. Haworth, Helen Hook, W. Isaacs, G. Lingle, Alice Meyer, F. O'Brien, R. Patterson, and Carol Shep- ard. The apparatuses were constructed by D. Sauer- brunn. Reprints may be obtained from either author, Behavior Research Laboratory, Anna State Hospital, Anna, Illinois 62906.
and Klosowski, 1965; Hundziak et al., 1965; Kimbrell et al., 1967). Attempts have been made to train normal independent toileting (Bensgerg et al., 1965; Giles and Wolf, 1966; Van Wagenen et al., 1969) but quantitative data regarding the enduring success of these attempts have not been given. In all of these studies, the post-training data were either ab- sent or too incomplete to determine the de- gree of the continence that endured after the training, as was also concluded by Rentfrow and Rentfrow (1969) in a recent review. At present, no procedure of established effective- ness is available for producing and maintain- ing normal independent toileting by institu- tionalized retardates. The overall rationale used in the present
effort was that normal toileting is not simply a matter of learning to respond to bladder and bowel pressures by relaxing the spincter but rather is a complex operant and social learning process that has been hindered by a reduced learning capacity and by institution- alization. In line with this rationale, a general procedure was developed in which positive reinforcement was given for appropriate elimi- nation and inhibitory training for untidiness (see also Van Wagenen et al., 1969; Giles and Wolf, 1966). Urine-sensing apparatuses were
89
1971, 49 89-99 NUMBER 2 (SUMMER 1971)
N. H. AZRIN and R. M. FOXX
used to provide feedback to the trainer such that he could then deliver the reinforcers or inhibitors immediately after the eliminatory response (see also Mowrer and Mowrer, 1938; Van Wagenen and Murdock, 1966; Watson, 1968; Van Wagenen et al., 1969; Azrin et al., in press). The operant level of urination (Van Wegenen et al., 1969) or defecation (see Giles and Wolf, 1966) was increased to provide more responses that could be reinforced, rather than dealing with the low frequency of elimination. Generalization of the effect of training was enhanced by conducting the training on the patient's own ward (see Giles and Wolf, 1966). Dressing and undressing skills and un- prompted approach to the toilet were taught as part of training (see Giles and Wolf, 1966; Van Wegenen et al, 1969). These procedures were modified and developed in this study for the intended effect of rapidity and complete- ness of continence. In addition, administrative procedures were arranged for assuring the con- tinued social concern of the ward staff so that the effects of the training would endure.
METHOD
Subjects Nine male incontinent and profoundly re-
tarded residents were selected from a hospital ward; the other residents of that ward were either continent, totally blind, or non-ambula- tory. The nine had a mean age of 43 yr and a range of 20 to 62 yr. Their mean number of years of hospitalization was 21 and ranged from 6 to 45 yr. All but one had an IQ less than 30; the median was 14 with a range of 7 to 45. Several had major physical and medi- cal problems, e.g., two had paralysis of one arm and one was partially blind. Speech was minimal for three residents and absent for five; one was echolalaic. All had limited re- ceptive speech but could follow simple di- rections. Virtually no interaction was observed between residents on the ward of either a pos- itive or negative nature. The residents were regularly toileted as part of the ward routine after each meal. Despite these regularly sched- uled periods, incontinence remained the ma- jor ward problem. Habit training procedures had been used previously with these residents but had not produced bladder and bowel con- trol or independent toileting.
Experimental Design A three-day baseline measure of inconti-
nence was obtained for each resident, after which the nine residents were randomly as- signed to a treatment or control group, the two groups being matched on the basis of the average number of accidents for the group. The experimental group of four residents was trained as a group. When the last experi- mental resident completed training, after 12 days, training began for the control group (N = 5). Four of the five residents in the con- trol group began training as a group, the fifth being added as soon as one of the four com- pleted training. As each of the nine residents completed training, they were placed on the post-training maintenance program. The matching of groups permitted a standard between-groups statistical evaluation of the effects of toilet training. The later training of the control group permitted a within-subject evaluation for all residents by comparing the pre- and post-training accidents for each of the nine.
Recording All previous reports of incontinence have
based the accident data on the retrospective verbal reports or uncorroborated written re- ports of parents or attendants who presumably recorded accidents on an unknown basis of selective attention at different times. In the present study, the resident was observed every hour for 8 hr each day and a written record taken at that moment by an assigned observer as to whether the pants were wet or soiled. The recording procedure was kept constant for the three-day period preceding training and the first 12 days after training for each resident. Any accidents observed between the hourly checks were also recorded. When pants were wet or soiled, they were changed immed- iately so that successive observations of wet pants did reflect repeated wettings. Twelve days after the last resident was trained, the recording procedure was changed to be more compatible with the continence maintenance procedures described below. The residents were individually observed six times per day at roughly 2.5-hr intervals by an assigned ob- server. Corroboration of the records was con- ducted through several methods. Reliability was assessed on the third day of the baseline
90
TOILET TRAINING OF THE ADULT RETARDED
period and twice during the 12-day post-train- ing period by having the trainer and a ward staff member concurrently observe the resi- dents with each observer unaware of the other's records; perfect agreement was ob- tained. The ward supervisors regularly ob- served the residents during the maintenance period, especially at the designated observa- tion hours and indicated in writing their con- firmation or disagreement with the data re- corded. Virtually all such reports were in agreement. Intermittent observations by the trainer on the ward corroborated the occur- rence of the supervisory as well as the regular observations. The record sheets were located in the supervisor's office and changed each day to discourage retrospective or unsupervised recording. Table 1 is a listing of the various steps of
the toilet training procedure.
Table 1 Outline of the Toilet Training Procedure
I. When No Accidents Occur 1) Resident seated in chair when not seated on
toilet bowl 2) Resident drinks fluids every half-hour 3) Scheduled toileting of resident every half-hour 4) Resident given edible and social reinforcer
every 5 min while dry 5) Shaping of undressing and dressing during toi-
leting 6) Resident given edible and social reinforcer fol-
lowing elimination in toilet bowl and returned to chair.
II. When Accidents Occur 1) Trainer disconnects pants alarm 2) Trainer obtains resident's attention 3) Resident walks to laundry area to obtain fresh
clothing 4) Resident undresses himself 5) Resident walks to nearby shower, receives
shower, and dresses himself 6) Resident obtains mop or cloth and cleans soiled
area on chair or floor 7) Resident handwashes soiled pants, wrings pants
out, and hangs pants up to dry 8) Trainer removes resident's chair from use 9) 1-hour timeout procedures:
a) no edibles or social reinforcers every 5 min; b) no fluids every 30 min; c) chair not available; d) continue 30-min scheduled toilet periods.
Apparatus Two apparatuses were employed during the
toilet training phase. The first, the "pants- alarm" apparatus illustrated in Fig. 1, is de-
scribed in detail in the report by Azrin et al. (in press). Briefly, that apparatus consisted of a moisture-sensitive pair of shorts that sounded a signal when the resident urinated or defecated in the shorts. A wire connected the sensors in the shorts to a small circuit box worn on a belt under the resident's shirt. The circuit box sounded a bleep-tone whenever the residents urinated or defecated in the shorts as the moisture completed a low voltage circuit that was far below the threshold of feeling. The second apparatus detected appropriate
toileting and was a slight modification of the "toilet-chair" apparatus described in Azrin et al. (in press). The present apparatus (shown in Fig. 2) was a plastic bowl that was inserted in the ward toilet bowls. The malleability of the plastic allowed the plastic bowl to fit the con- tour of the ward toilet bowl and allowed the hinged toilet seat to rest normally over the toilet bowl. The plastic bowls were commer- cially available in a variety of sizes; a size was selected that matched closely the interior di- mensions of the ward toilet bowl. The plastic bowl was heat-formed to provide a slight de- pression at the center in which were fastened
WET-ALARM PANTS
BACK VI EW.
FRONT VIEW
CIRCUIT I / / BOX B BELLT
MOISTURE DETECTING SNAPS
Fig. 1. The wet alarm pants are shown as viewed from the back and front of the wearer. The moisture detecting snaps seen in the front view were two cloth- ing snaps fastened to ordinary men's briefs. Two flex- ible wires are shown (back view) leading from the snaps to the circuit box. The snaps on the end of the wire were manually removable from the snaps on the clothing. The circuit box was worn on a belt (back view) which was worn "high" on the abdomen. Normal trousers were worn over the special training pants. A tone is sounded by the circuit box when urine or feces moistens the area between the clothing snaps.
91
N. H. AZRIN and R. M. FOXX
TOILET SIGN,
PLASTIC
MOISTURE/ DETECTING
STUDS
Fig. 2. The toilet signal arrangement i a side view. The malleable plastic bow] normal toilet bowl and rests on its top ed level (not shown) of the stool must be be tic bowl to avoid shorting of the moist studs on the bottom of the plastic bowl signed to be shorted by urine or feces lines are the detachable wires that conr to the circuit box which can rest on t circuit box sounds a tone when moistu two studs.
two metal sensing screws about 1 apart. Urine or feces in the bowl electrical signal to pass between the sors. Wires connected these metal the signal circuit box, which was that described in the report by Azr press), and which sounded a sig trainer. Immediacy of detection of elimi
inforcement is known to be mc when given immediately and fo sponse. The two automatic device immediacy possible by signalling a of elimination be they appropriate priate. The trainer was not forced resident continuously to the exclusi activities. The apparatus allowed mediate consequences while still the trainer: (1) to train several i once, (2) to leave the toilet facilit when showering one resident, and., to return quickly, (3) to teach th responses related to toilet trainii having to check the pants of other and (4) to eliminate looking betwc of a resident, sitting on the toilet, t( if he was voiding (see Watson, 196
"We have been informed by Lehigh Va ics Inc. that they can supply both of th described here at an individual cost a Address: Box 125, Fogelsville, Pa. 18051.
AL Preventing behaviors incompatible with toileting. Failure of a resident to go to the ward toilet could be caused by the inconve- nience of interrupting other behaviors such as sitting in a preferred chair (see Baumeister and Klosowki, 1965). In order to prevent such behavioral incompatibility, the resident was required to remain in the ward toilet area during the entire 8-hr training session. To
CIRCUIT reduce further the effort required in toileting, _ BOX the residents were seated within one meter of
the toilet bowls. Residents remained in the is shown from toilet during the entire daily session of about I fits into the 8 hr except when they ate their lunch at a Ige. The water table just outside the toilet. The area around elow the plas. the toilet was visually partitioned off to reduceture detecting which are de- still further any competing reinforcers arising s. The dotted from distractions by other residents. Since iect the studs sitting in the chair could be considered as he floor. The still somewhat incompatible with approach toire shorts the
the toilet bowls, this source of incompatibility was also eliminated for 1 hr when a resident
in. (2.5 cm) had an accident by removing the chair. I caused an Increasing the frequency of urinations. Uri- e metal sen- nation or defecation is a low frequency be- I sensors to havior under normal circumstances even for identical to incontinent individuals. Consequently, few re- rin et al. (in inforcers for correct toileting can be given ,nal to the during a day. To solve this problem, the pro-
cedure increased greatly the operant level of ination. Re- urinating by giving each resident as large a )st effective volume of fluids (coffee, tea, or water) to drink or every re- each half hour as he would consume. Raising s made this the frequency of urinations would provide Lll instances more frequent opportunities each day to re- or inappro- inforce correct toileting and to react nega- to watch a tively to incorrect toileting.
ion of other Stimulus control of elimination response. I fairly im- On the very first day of training, the procedure permitting attempted to habit train the residents, i.e., to
residents at have elimination occur shortly after the resi- .y briefly as dent sat on the toilet bowl. This stimulus con- still be able trol was attempted by physically arranging for ie residents almost all urinations to be associated with ng without the act of sitting on the bowl. The residents individuals, were required to sit on the toilet every half ten the legs hour and to remain there for 20 min or until determine an appropriate elimination was signalled by
;8). the toilet alarm, whichever occurred first. Elimination should, therefore, become associ- the next toileting. If a prompt was not effec-
lley Electron- tv h oedrciepopswr ie ie apparatuses tive, the more directive prompts were given f about $40. successively within a few seconds of each other
until one was effective. This dressing instruc-
92
TOILET TRAINING OF THE ADULT RETARDED
ated with the toilet stimuli, and because of the reinforcement of appropriate elimination there, the resident should start to eliminate immediately upon sitting upon the toilet. Im- mediate elimination upon being seated on the bowl, accompanied by no "accidents" when not seated, would indicate that simple habit training had been achieved.
Positive reinforcement of correct toileting. To increase the probability of toileting in the bowl, many probable reinforcers were given at the time of correct toileting and as long as the resident remained dry. When an appro- priate urination was signalled by the toilet bowl apparatus, the resident was given a large piece of a candy bar, hugged, and praised. To provide reinforcement for remaining dry, the residents were given edibles consisting of sugar-frosted cereal and M & M candies and simultaneously praised for having dry pants every 5 min for as long as they remained dry. The social reinforcement by verbal praise was made as continuous as the trainer's time sched- ule permitted within these 5-min intervals. Another source of reinforcement for being dry was the drink that was given every half-hour for the purpose of raising the operant level of urinations as described above.
Dressing skills. One of the essential responses in the chain of events involved in independent toileting is the act of pulling the pants down before eliminating and pulling them up again after eliminating. Since several of the residents had been described by the ward staff as unable to dress themselves, this inability would be expected to prevent independent toileting. The general procedure, therefore, included a provision for teaching this skill. When the res- ident approached the toilet bowl, the trainer used the minimal prompt that was effective in causing the resident to undress, beginning with no prompt at all, then pointing at the pants, then verbal instruction, touching the pants, placing the resident's hands on his pants, and lastly, guiding the resident's hands, if necessary, through the entire act. The same fading of prompts was given for pulling the pants up and for flushing the toilet bowl after elimination. When a prompt was effective, the trainer used the next less directive prompt on tion was given at each half-hour scheduled toileting, as .well as at any self-initiated toilet- ings. Social approval and edibles were given, as necessary, for completion of the individual
steps in the dressing sequence but were discon- tinued once the total sequence was established and was followed by correct elimination in the bowl. Independent toilet approach (self-initiation
of toileting). For long-term institutionalized residents, toilet-approach has often been ini- tiated by external prompting stimuli. Attend- ants often instructed, accompanied, led, or physically seated the resident on the toilet seat. To eliminate the dependence on the prompting stimuli, the trainer deliberately used the minimal social prompt that was effec- tive and progressively reduced these prompts on successive trials until none were given. If necessary, the trainer actively "molded" the resident's limbs through the desired act and verbally instructed him to perform that act. This direct verbal and/or physical guidance was then reduced to a touch, then to a hand motion, then a faint head or finger motion, then nothing. The fading of prompts was be- gun from the start of training. If the resident did not respond to a mild prompt, a more visible prompt was given within a few seconds, and if necessary a still stronger one, up to the last step of physical and verbal guidance until he was seated. Once the social prompts were minimal or absent, the proprioceptive stimuli, the negative consequences of soiling, and the positive reinforcement for toileting should be- come the exclusive sources of control. When the resident approached the toilet for the first time without a prompt and toileted himself, all further prompts were discontinued even though the fading process had been only par- tially completed up to that time. To reduce further inadvertent social prompts once the fading was complete, the trainer discontinued the edibles and social approval that he had been giving every 5 min that the resident was dry. The trainer continued to bestow the edi- ble and social approval when the resident eliminated correctly in the toilet bowl. The anticipation of positive reinforcement and the avoidance of disapproval would become the basis for approaching the toilet bowl. Modelling. Training could be expected to
be more rapid if imitative learning could be used in addition to the shaping. If the resi- dents were sensitive to the actions of others, they could be expected to learn faster by watching the other residents toilet correctly. This modelling influence was arranged by de-
93
N. H. AZRIN and R. M. FOXX
liberately training several residents at one time so they could observe each other. To maximize interpatient observation, the chairs of the residents were placed close together in close proximity to the toilet bowls so there was ample opportunity for residents to view other residents moving toward, or elimi- nating in, the toilet and receiving reinforce- ment.
Inhibitions of "accidents". Urinating in the pants was inhibited by verbal reprimands and by timeout from positive reinforcement. When the pa its-alarm apparatus signalled an acci- dent, the resident was bodily shaken to gain his attention and told not to wet his pants. The resident was then given the following cleanliness training. He was immediately taken to the nearby shower stall where he un- dressed and was given a tepid shower. Follow- ing the shower, the resident was required to change his clothes himself and to carry the soiled clothes to a sink, to completely immerse them in water, wring them out and hang them up to dry. Upon arriving back at the toilet, the resident was required to mop up the floor or wipe from his chair any traces of his soiling. For the residents that were reluctant to per- form these activities, the trainer manually guided the residents arms and hands through the proper motions and faded out this manual guidance as the resident began to cooperate. For 1 hr following the accident, a timeout pe- riod was in effect during which the resident re- ceived none of the drinks, no edibles, no social reinforcement, and a delay of the regularly scheduled portion of the lunch.
Post-Training Maintenance Procedure In accordance with the present conception
of proper toileting as socially motivated, the benefits of toilet training were not expected to endure after training unless the ward staff was sufficiently concerned to continue to en- courage proper toileting and to discourage accidents. For administrative considerations, such a maintenance procedure ideally should be easily conducted, require only one attend- ant, require little time, be meaningful to the residents and be capable of supervision by the ward supervisors without any special staffing.
Table 2 is an outline of the procedures used during the post-training ward maintenance program. The procedure established six oc- casions when a resident would be inspected for
incontinence: entrance to the dining room (three occasions), between-meal snack periods (two occasions) and bedtime (one occasion). At each inspection period, the attendant encour- aged continence by praising the resident if he was dry, discouraging incontinence when he was wet by reprimanding him, requiring the cleanliness training (described above), and either delaying slightly the resident's entrance to the forthcoming meal or omitting the snack during snack periods.
Table 2 Post Training Ward Maintenance Procedure
I. General Procedure 1) Advance assignment of one attendant for Toilet
Responsibility each shift 2) Snack period between breakfast and lunch and
between lunch and dinner 3) Residents pants inspected at mealtime, snack-
time and bedtime (6 times daily) 4) Attendant initials record sheet when residents
checked; record sheet sent directly to supervisor 5) Discontinued use of both apparatuses for detect-
ing eliminations. II. When Accidents Occur
1) Cleanliness training whenever an accident was detected: a) Resident walks to laundry area to obtain
fresh clothing b) Resident undresses himself c) Resident walks to nearby shower, receives
shower and dresses himself d) Resident obtains mop or cloth and cleans
soiled area on chair or floor e) Resident handwashes soiled pants, wrings
pants out, and hangs pants up to dry 2) Delay of meal for 1 hr if accident prior to meal 3) Omission of snacks if accident prior to snack 4) Attendant initials and records each accident
Minimal Maintenance - Starts Eight Weeks after Training
1) Inspections only at mealtime and bedtime 2) Cleanliness training given for accidents
Termination of Maintenance Procedure - When resi- dent is continent for at least one month.
1) No regular inspections for that patient 2) Cleanliness training given for accidents when
detected
To insure administrative feasibility and ad- equacy of supervision by the normal super- visory personnel, several procedures were in- stituted. Discussion with the ward supervising nurse, the ward treatment program director, and the nursing division director showed that all were concerned that the residents should remain continent. To translate this concern into action, toileting responsibility was as-
94
TOILET TRAINING OF THE ADULT RETARDED
signed to a specific attendant each shift, a different attendant being assigned on different days. The assignments would be made a month in advance, by specific day, with an alternate attendant also scheduled should the assigned attendant be absent or indisposed. A daily pre- dated record sheet was designed on which the attendant responsible for the six inspections would sign his name each time an inspection was made. To insure adequacy of supervision by the concerned treatment supervisors, this sheet was then checked by the ward supervis- ing nurse who then initialled it, thereby sig- nifying her assurance that all inspections had been completed by the attendant specified for that shift. She in turn routed the check sheet to the ward treatment director who also ini- tialled it and in turn routed it to the nursing division director. The nursing division direc- tor returned the sheets with written comments which were then discussed by the ward super- visor during the weekly staff meetings where all attendants were normally present. A second record sheet to record the specifics
of the residents accidents was placed in the ward clothing room. The attendant noted on this record the resident's name, time found wet, whether cleanliness training was given, and whether a meal was delayed, or a snack missed. The attendant who was assigned the toileting responsibility was to record any ac- cidents during his shift, even those brought to his attention by another attendant. One month after training, a minimal main-
tenance procedure was instituted during which the procedure was simplified by omit- ting the two extra between-meal snacks and the between-meal inspection periods. Only the mealtime and bedtime inspections were held. When four weeks had elapsed without a
single accident for a given resident, the pro- gram was discontinued for that resident. He was no longer inspected at the regular inspec- tion periods; cleanliness training was contin- ued, however, should an accident occur.
RESULTS All accidents throughout the day had been
recorded. Only the accidents recorded within the 8-hr period from 8:00 A.M. to 4:00 P.M. are included in the data presentation, however, because maximum supervision of the record- ing procedure occurred at that time and be-
cause the baseline records included only that period of time. Reduction of incontinence. Figure 3 shows
the number of accidents before and after train- ing for all nine residents. Before training, the residents averaged about two accidents per 8-hr day per patient. After training, the num- ber of accidents decreased to about one acci- dent every fourth day per resident. The de- crease was immediate and endured for the entire follow-up period. By the fifth month after training, accidents were virtually absent. A one-tailed t test was used to compare the
accidents during the 12-day post-training pe- riod of the treatment group with the com- parable no-training period of the control group, based on the difference from the base- line period. The number of accidents was sig- nificantly lower for the treatment group (t = 3.93, df = 7, P < 0.001). The mean number of accidents per resident changed from 2.1 to 0.2 for the treatment group and from 1.9 to 2.3 for the control group. Examination of the records for each resident
revealed that before training, the most incon- tinent resident averaged four accidents per day, whereas the least incontinent resident averaged one every three days. For each of the nine residents the number of accidents was re- duced by 80% or more during the first 12 days after training. Two residents accounted for almost all accidents one month after training. A within-subject comparison of the pre- and
post-training accidents of all nine residents by the Wilcoxin Matched-Pairs Signed-Ranks Test (Siegel, 1956) showed a significant (P < 0.01) reduction of accidents for each week of the post-training period. Although the majority of accidents were
urinations, defecation was also affected by training. Prior to training, an average of 2.0 pants defecations (soilings) occurred per day for the nine residents combined. During the first seven weeks of the post-training period, they averaged 0.1 soilings, a reduction of over 90%.
Results during training. Training required a median of four days and a mean of six days, and ranged from 1 to 14 days. These figures include all daily training sessions from habit training through the last day of independent toileting training. An average of 25 cups of fluid were con-
sumed per resident per day of training. Dur-
95
N. H. AZRIN and R. M. FOXX
2.0~
_ ,,,TOI LE T en F oTRAININGTRAIN.5NG N:9 RETARDED ADULTSzQ< 1.5
UJ XUJ4> 41.0
001° Xz LLW
0*5
so -3-1 +1 7 14 21 28 35 42 49 56 84 112 140
DAYS Fig. 3. The effect of the toilet training procedure on the frequency of accidents. The accidents include def-
ecations as well as urinations in the clothing. The subjects were nine adult profoundly retarded residents. The toilet training period is shown as an interruption in the curve. The three data points before the toilet training show the three-day baseline period before training. The abscissa is labelled in terms of the number of days elap- sing after training was terminated. Data points are given for each of the three days prior, and seven days sub- sequent, to the toilet training period. Thereafter, each data point is the average number of accidents per day for a seven-day period.
ing the training the residents averaged 15 uri- nations per day and one bowel movement every third day per resident.
Five of the nine residents seemed to possess voluntary control over their bladders and bowels (were under the stimulus control of the toilet) at the very start of training. This voluntary bladder control was indicated by: (1) no accidents in the pants during the entire first day, (2) almost immediate elimination upon sitting on the toilet if elimination oc- curred at all, and (3) external signs that the resident was straining to eliminate in the toilet. Each of the other four remaining resi- dents had at least two accidents in their pants, and consistently remained on the toilet seat for extended periods without eliminating. Training of voluntary control, as evidenced by fairly immediate elimination upon sitting on the toilet seat, was accomplished for all residents within three days. The existence of voluntary control over
elimination did not assure independent toilet- ing. For example, three of the residents who exhibited some voluntary control on the very first day of training required about nine addi- tional days before they consistently and in- dependently toileted themselves without ac- cidents. Apparatus usage and scheduling. The
trainer was able to train four residents simul- taneously (but three residents at a time were a more comfortable number with which to work). The number of available toilet bowls was not as critical, since a bowl was rarely in- disposed for very long. Since three bowls were available for the four residents, the toileting periods were scheduled in a staggered time se- quence. Four pants-alarm apparatuses and three toilet-bowl-alarm apparatuses were used. Both apparatuses were highly reliable, as initial "bugs" in the apparatus had been worked out in an earlier study (Azrin et al., in press). A false positive occurred (alarm
96
TOILET TRAINING OF THE ADULT RETARDED
sounded when no wetting had occurred) on the first day of training because of excessive perspiration; this problem was solved by plac- ing a small piece of adhesive tape over the back of the snaps. The tape remained in place throughout the training study, even through repeated laundering of the briefs.
Reliable operation of the toilet-alarm ap- paratus required only that the bowls were emptied and the moisture-detecting studs wiped dry after each usage during training. As noted previously, neither apparatus was used after training. Ward staff and patient reaction. Many of
the ward staff were pessimistic about the like- lihood of success of the proposed program, probably because of their experience with other types of efforts. After observing the resi- dents mopping up their own accidents, and independently initiating toileting for the first time, the majority of the ward staff volun- teered statements of satisfaction. The staff began asking the trainer for recommendations for treatment programs unrelated to toilet training for other ward residents. The dra- matic reduction (about 90%) in incontinence following the training has been commented on favorably by the majority of the ward staff and all the supervisors.
Patient reaction. The question was raised by several ward staff members as to how the residents would react to the 8 hr per day in- tensive training sessions. This question could not be answered by the nonverbal residents, but the staff consensus was that almost all be- havioral signs showed the program in almost every case to be a very pleasant attention- getting experience. During the training, the residents obtained an unusually higher dens- ity of positive reinforcement in the form of both food, drinks, verbal praise, hugs, and attention. Most residents would not leave the toilet at the end of the daily training sessions even when asked to leave. Two of the residents who had completed training and were in the ward maintenance program repeatedly at- tempted to reenter the training area even though the toilet area was partitioned off from the rest of the ward. At no time did any of the residents physically aggress toward the trainer. Only two residents attempted to leave the toilet, and then only during some of the time- out periods. One of these residents seemed to be delib-
erately resisting any attempt to make him con- tinent, even though he had an excellent volun- tary bladder control. He was both the major incontinence problem on the ward and also the highest functioning of the residents (esti- mated IQ-45). The trainer was required to give an unusually large amount of manual guidance, attention, and encouragement to this resident who occupied more training time than any other resident.
Transition from training to maintenance. The proper conduct of the maintenance pro- cedure was facilitated by having the trainer teach the ward staff. During the first two days, the trainer directly assisted the staff. During succeeding days, the trainer observed the in- spection periods and answered many questions about the maintenance procedures, such as when and how to inspect the resident, division of staff responsibility, method of changing res- ident, etc. After this training of the staff, no administrative problem existed in having the staff spend the small amount of time needed to inspect the resident and to have the residents change and clean themselves when necessary, especially since so few accidents were now occurring.
Six weeks after training, five residents were discontinued from the Ward Maintenance Program since they had not had an accident for over four weeks. Nine weeks after train- ing, two more residents were discontinued from the maintenance program. The Ward Maintenance Program was dropped in the middle of the fifth month after the two re- maining residents were continent for four weeks. The ward attendant in charge of the cloth-
ing room reported that since completion of training the number of pants sent to the hospital laundry each week had been reduced by over 40%. Night time incontinence. No training re-
garding night time incontinence had been given. Yet, several reports were made by the night shift that the residents involved in toilet training were getting up at night to eliminate. This result, although not a general occurrence, indicated that some degree of generalization had occurred as a result of training.
DISCUSSION The results showed that the present pro-
cedure virtually eliminated daytime incon-
97
N. H. AZRIN and R. M. FOXX
tinence in all residents. Training was rapid, requiring only a few days (median of four days) for each resident. The reduction en- dured indefinitely and required little staff time to maintain. Bowel incontinence was re- duced to the same extent as bladder incon- tinence with no need for a differential training procedure. Continence was achieved even for individual retardates with IQs as low as 7, ex- treme physical disabilities, as much as 45 yr of institutional incontinence, deliberate re- fusal to be continent, no language, as old as 62 yr of age, locomotor and manipulative dis- abilities, and no indication of other self-help skills. The special training apparatuses re- quired were of low cost and reliable. The training procedure was not unpleasant to the residents or to the staff; the maintenance pro- cedure for insuring continued continence was easily performed by the ward staff. This latter consideration of hospital feasibility probably ranks in importance with the effectiveness of the procedure as a determinant of the practical value of the program for institutions. The reduction of incontinence seems to
have resulted from the training procedure and not from other factors. The experimental de- sign used both a within-subjects and a be- tween-groups comparison. The within-subjects comparison assured that the effect could not have resulted from chance assignment of spon- taneously continent residents to a treatment group. The between-groups design provided a control for time or other events occurring in time, since the control group was remaining incontinent at the same time that the treat- ment group had become continent. Thus, staff expectations, resident expectations, the inspec- tion periods, the renewed concern regarding continence, the presence of the trainer, daily inspection of resident's clothing, resident's diet, etc., were all held statistically compar- able for the treatment and the no-treatment phases. The distinctive feature of this training pro-
cedure was its consideration of proper toilet- ing as a complex and lengthy chain of re- sponses that includes social, physical, and physiological stimuli and requires strong posi- tive and negative operant consequences for its maintenance in that chain, rather than con- sidering it as a simple associative muscular reflex to internal stimuli. This analysis is in ac- cord with the Ellis (1963) conception of toi-
leting as an operant response that is susceptible to training by operant reinforcement that pro- duces stimulus control by exteroceptive stim- uli as well as bladder stimuli. Eliminating in a toilet bowl rather than on the floor or in one's clothing is a social convention that requires social consequences. Yet, the lack of concern by otherwise preoccupied attendants often re- sults in an absence or delay of these social consequences. Consequently, the procedure as- sured that the concerned institutional treat- ment supervisors did have feedback about the attendant's behavior and could, thereby, com- municate their concern to all attendants and encourage their participation. Also, the pro- cedure was based on the rationale that inde- pendent toileting usually is not instigated by demanding bladder or bowel pressures, as sug- gested by the simple reflex-arc orientation, but rather by the negative results (shame, guilt) of having visibly soiled clothing. Consequently, the procedure established stronger-than-nor- mal negative consequences such as cleanliness training, and the delay of meals. Similarly, during training, the signalling apparatus was used to obtain the trainer's immediate reac- tion to the act of elimination, not to provide feedback to the resident who was already aware that he had eliminated. Postulation of the importance of the environmental and soc- ial influences, rather than simply the bladder influences suggested that generalization of the toilet training to the resident's natural en- vironment would be more effective if con- ducted in that very environment and by the very ward attendants normally present there. Recognition of continence as a lengthy oper- ant chain of toilet behaviors suggested that in- tensive concern should be given to the resi- dent's skill in dressing as a major contributing factor as well as to his unprompted approach behavior to the toilet and for the absence of competing reinforcers of inactivity. The over- all objective was to teach and motivate the resident to toilet himself even when bladder pressures were minimal. Thus, the present ra- tionale conceptualized continence as a com- plex operant reaction to social factors rather than as an associative reaction of a single mus- cle to internal stimuli. The present training procedure offers a
method of eliminating incontinence of severe and profound adult retardates. No effective alternative has been reported for the adult
98
TOILET TRAINING OF THE ADULT RETARDED 99
retarded. The time necessary for training was much less (median of four days) than has been reported for training other types of popula- tions. A training procedure for incontinent psychotics required about four weeks for a re- duction of incontinence of about 50% (Wag- ner and Paul, 1970); a procedure for young retardates required a median of six weeks with generalization data to the institutional ward incomplete (Giles and Wolf, 1966); a proce- dure for outpatient retarded children has re- quired a median of 12 days, but the follow-up data for the home situation was incomplete (Van Wagenen et al., 1969). Several procedures have been reported for producing voluntary bladder or bowel control for young retardates but did not produce independent or unassisted toileting. A procedure for achieving this blad- der training phase required about four weeks (Hundziak et al., 1965), about nine weeks (Baumeister and Klosowski, 1965) and about 29 weeks (Kimbrell et al., 1967). As compared with the alternatives, the present training pro- cedure seemed to require far less time, re- duces incontinence to a greater extent, leads to complete independent toileting, generalizes more to the resident's natural setting, and shows no regression during follow-up.
REFERENCES Azrin, N. H., Bugle, C., and O'Brien, F. Behavioral
engineering: two apparatuses for use in toilet train- ing retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis (in press).
Baumeister, A. and Klosowski, R. An attempt to group toilet train severely retarded patients. Mental Retardation, 1965, 3, 24-26.
Bensberg, G. J., Colwell, C. N., and Cassell, R. H.
Teaching the profoundly retarded self help skill activities by behavior-shaping techniques. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1965, 69, 674-679.
Dayan, M. Toilet training retarded children in a state residential institution. Mental Retardation, 1964, 2, 116-117.
Ellis, N. R. Toilet training and the severely defective patient: an S-R reinforcement analysis. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1963, 68, 98-103.
Giles, D. K. and Wolf, M. M. Toilet training institu- tionalized, severe retardates: an application of be- havior modification techniques. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1966, 70, 766-780.
Hundziak, M., Maurer, R. A., and Watson, L. S. Op- erant conditioning in toilet training severely men- tally retarded boys. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1965, 70, 120-124.
Kimbrell, D. L., Luckey, R. E., Barbuto, P. F. P., and Love, J. G. Operation dry pants: an intensive habit-training program for severely and profoundly retarded. Mental Retardation, 1967, 5, 32-36.
Mowrer, 0. H. and Mowrer, W. M. Enuresis: a method for its study and treatment. American Jour- nal of Orthopsychiatry, 1938, 8, 436-459.
Rentfrow, R. K. and Rentfrow, D. K. Studies re- lated to toilet training of the mentally retarded. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 1969, 23, 425-430.
Siegel, S. Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1956.
Van Wagenen, R. K., Meyerson, L., Kerr, N. J., and Mahoney, K. Field trials of a new procedure for toilet training. Journal of Experimental Child Psy- chology, 1969, 8, 147-159.
Van Wagenen, R. K. and Murdock, E. E. A transis- torized signal-package for toilet training of infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1966, 3, 312-314.
Wagner, B. R. and Paul, G. L. Reduction of incon- tinence in chronic mental patients: a pilot project. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 1970, 1, 29-38.
Watson, L. S., Jr. Application of behavior-shaping devices to training severely and profoundly men- tally retarded children in an institutional setting. Mental Retardation, 1968, 6, 21-23.