Loading...

Messages

Proposals

Stuck in your homework and missing deadline? Get urgent help in $10/Page with 24 hours deadline

Get Urgent Writing Help In Your Essays, Assignments, Homeworks, Dissertation, Thesis Or Coursework & Achieve A+ Grades.

Privacy Guaranteed - 100% Plagiarism Free Writing - Free Turnitin Report - Professional And Experienced Writers - 24/7 Online Support

Multi causal model of addiction

18/12/2020 Client: saad24vbs Deadline: 7 Days

e eBook Collection


25


CHAPTER 2


The Etiology of Addiction


Almost everyone has an easy answer to the


question: Why do people use drugs? According


to Stewart (1987), heroin addicts


use “junk” the fi rst time because they are curious.


Heroin has a mystique. It is used by pop stars, writers,


and glamorous people, and they like its effect.


For those who fi nd daily life to be fairly humdrum,


heroin can be the ultimate filler of gaps—it can


substitute for career, religion, romance, or virtually


anything else. Weil and Rosen (1993) believe


that drug use (and addiction) results from humans’


longing for a sense of completeness and wholeness,


and searching for satisfaction outside of


themselves. As noted author (and addict) William


S. Burroughs (1977) indicated in Junky, “Junk


wins by default. I tried it as a matter of curiosity.


I drifted along taking shots when I could score. I


ended up hooked” (p. xv). This notion of drift is a


recurrent theme in theories of addiction.


People begin using cocaine for some of the


same reasons. According to Baum (1985), his clients


provided these excuses for using cocaine:


“The mystical reputation aroused my curiosity.” . . .


“It’s available and being offered all the time.” . . .


“It gave me a sense of well-being, like I was worth


something.” . . .


“It felt good to be a part of a group.” . . .


“It was a great way to escape.” (pp. 25–42)


The reasons why people continue to use drugs


to the point of becoming physically and/or psychologically


dependent on them are more complex.


Some have attempted to explain this phenomenon


as a defi cit in moral values, a disease, conditioning


or learned behavior, or as a genetic propensity.


Still others see it as a “rewiring” of the brain (see


Chapter 3 ). At this point, there is no one single


theory that adequately explains addiction.


Jacobs (1986) attempted to develop a general


theory of addiction, drawing on his experience


and research with gamblers. In his view, addiction


is a dependent state acquired over time to


relieve stress. Two interrelated sets of factors are


required to predispose persons to addiction: an abnormal


physiological resting state, and childhood


experiences producing a deep sense of inadequacy.


He argues that all addictions (drugs, sex,


alcohol, etc.) follow a similar three-stage course of


development.


Most models of addiction assume that an addiction


is an “addictive disease” (Washton, 1989,


p. 55). As such, it continues to exist whether or


not the addicted person continues to use the drug.


Even if a person who has the disease is abstinent for


a long period of time, the symptoms of addiction


will appear again from renewed contact with the


drug. The disease model of addiction rests on three


primary assumptions: predisposition to use a drug,


loss of control over use, and progression (Krivanek,


1988, p. 202). Johnson (1973) put it somewhat


differently in saying, “The most signifi cant characteristics


of the disease [ alcoholism ] are that it is primary,


progressive, chronic, and fatal” (p. 1). There


are others, such as Peele (1985), who question the


validity of this model. Speaking of the complex


nature of addiction, he rejects all strictly biological


explanations and says that addiction cannot


ISBN 1-256-72583-8


Chemical Dependency: A Systems Approach, Fourth Edition, by C. Aaron McNeece and Diana M. DiNitto. Published by Pearson. Copyright © 2010 by Pearson Education, Inc.


26 PA R T O N E ● Theories, Models, and Definitions


be resolved biologically because “lived human


experience and its interpretation are central to


the incidence, course, treatment, and remission


of addiction (see Preface).” An adequate theory


would have to synthesize pharmacological, experiential,


cultural, situational, and personality


components.


Drummond (2001) provides an interesting


perspective on theories of drug craving, most


of which can be classified into three categories:


(1) phenomenological models, which are based on


clinical observation and description; (2) conditioning


or cue-reactivity models, which are useful in the


exploration of craving and relapse; and (3) cognitive


models, which are based on social learning theory.


He concludes that no one theory provides an


adequate explanation of the phenomenon of craving.


Addiction, drug dependence, and craving are all


terms used to identify the phenomenon of loss of


control over drug-taking behavior, although each


has a slightly different meaning.


Etiological Theories


Addiction is not easily defi ned. For some, it involves


the “continued, self-administered use of a substance


despite substance-related problems, and it results


in tolerance for the substance, withdrawal from


the substance, and compulsive drug-taking behavior


due to cravings” or drives to use the substance


(Schuckit, 1992, p. 182). However, the American


Psychiatric Association’s criteria for dependence do


not require that tolerance or withdrawal be present


(see Chapter 5 ).


There are at least as many explanatory theories


of addiction as there are defi nitions. We will


focus on three broad theoretical categories—


psychological theories, biological theories, and


sociocultural theories—as well as discuss some


alternative explanations. These theories are not


mutually exclusive, and divisions sometimes


seem quite arbitrary. None is presented as the


correct way of explaining this phenomenon. We


do have preferences, and we lean more toward


certain models than others, but no single theory


adequately describes the etiology of addiction or


dependence. (For a more comprehensive treatment


of etiology, see Ott, Tarter, and Ammerman


[1999].) As social workers, it seems fitting to


suggest that the “person-in-environment” model


may ultimately provide the best mechanism for an


understanding of addiction. We will return to this


perspective at the end of the chapter.


The Moral Model


One of the earliest theories offered to explain the


etiology of addiction is humankind’s sinful nature.


Since it is diffi cult to show empirical evidence of


a sinful nature, the moral model of addiction has


been generally discredited by modern scholars.


However, the legacy of treating alcoholism and


drug addiction as sin or moral weakness continues


to infl uence public policies regarding alcohol and


drug abuse. Perhaps this is why needle/syringe


exchange programs have been so strongly opposed


in the United States.


Psychological Theories


Another explanation for the origins of craving alcohol


and mind-altering drugs lies in the psychological


literature—that is, the literature that deals with


one’s mind and emotions. Psychological models defi


ne addiction as an individual phenomenon but do


not necessarily exclude or minimize social factors


or other elements in the development of an addiction.


There are actually several different psychological


theories of alcoholism and drug addiction; they


include cognitive-behavioral, learning, psychodynamic,


and personality theories, among others.


Cognitive-Behavioral Theories. The cognitivebehavioral


theories offer a variety of motivations


for taking drugs. One such explanation states


that humans take drugs to experience variety


(Weil & Rosen, 1993). The need for variety is demonstrated


in cross-cultural expressions such as


singing, dancing, running, and joking. Drug use is


ISBN 1-256-72583-8


Chemical Dependency: A Systems Approach, Fourth Edition, by C. Aaron McNeece and Diana M. DiNitto. Published by Pearson. Copyright © 2010 by Pearson Education, Inc.


C H A P T E R 2 ● The Etiology of Addiction 27


an individual can avoid the unpleasant symptoms


of withdrawal. Repetitive action motivated by the


avoidance of unpleasant stimuli is called negative


reinforcement. (In an alcoholic, the need to avoid


withdrawal symptoms generally occurs from 6 to


48 hours after the last drink.) Another source of


negative reinforcement may lie in the avoidance of


unpleasant things other than withdrawal. There


is a high correlation between traumatic events


and subsequent substance abuse (Janoff-Bulman,


1992). The traumatized individual may take drugs


to avoid unpleasant memories or heightened physiological


states such as startle responses.


Learning Theory. Closely related to cognitivebehavioral


theories is learning, or reinforcement, theory.


Learning theory assumes that alcohol or drug


use results in a decrease in psychological states such


as anxiety, stress, and tension, thus positively reinforcing


the user. This learned response continues until


physical dependence develops, at which time the


aversion of withdrawal symptoms becomes a prime


motivation for drug use (Tarter & Schneider, 1976).


There is a considerable amount of evidence


to support that part of learning theory related to


alcohol use and physiological aversion. Abrupt


cessation of drinking will lead to unpleasant symptoms


of withdrawal (A & DRCC, 1995). For the


alcoholic, withdrawal can lead to trembling, shaking,


hallucinations, and tonic-clonic seizures, formerly


known as grand mal seizures. Similarly, for


the heroin addict, abrupt withdrawal may lead to


symptoms much like a case of severe fl u. In each


case, the addict quickly learns that these symptoms


may be avoided by resuming use of the drug.


An interesting view of becoming a heroin addict


is provided by Krivanek (1989). Dependencies


that involve drug use follow the same basic principles


of learning theory, as all other dependencies.


Krivanek views drug dependence as a psychological


phenomenon that can vary in intensity from


a mild involvement to an addiction that seriously


restricts the user’s other behaviors. Pattison,


Sobell, and Sobell (1977) view alcoholism as


a continuum. That is, “An individual’s use of


associated with a variety of activities—for example,


religious services, self-exploration, altering moods,


escaping boredom or despair, enhancing social


interaction, enhancing sensory experience or


pleasure, and stimulating creativity and performance.


A study on inner-city youths revealed that


youths are motivated to take drugs out of a desire


for variety, citing curiosity, celebration, getting


high, and rebelling as reasons for drug use. (In the


study, the youths celebrate or explore drugs by using


alcohol at home, whereas they choose to use


illegal marijuana away from the home [Esbensen


& Huizinga, 1990].) Assuming that people enjoy


variety, it follows that they repeat actions that


bring pleasure (positive reinforcement).


The desire to experience pleasure is another


cognitive explanation for drug use and abuse.


Some animals seek alcohol and even work for it (by


pushing a lever) to repeat a pleasant experience.


Alcohol and other drugs are chemical surrogates of


natural reinforcers such as eating, drinking, and


reproductive behavior. Social drinkers and alcoholics


both report using alcohol to relax, even though


tests of actual tension-reducing effects of alcohol


have yielded quite different results; scientific observations


of persons using alcohol actually show


them to become more depressed, anxious, and


nervous (NIAAA, 1996). The dependent behavior


is maintained by the degree of reinforcement the


alcohol provides, and this, in turn, depends on the


actor’s perception of his or her need hierarchy and


“the likelihood that this course of action will meet


the most important needs better than other available


options” (Krivanek, 1989, p. 96). Since alcohol


and drugs are more powerful and persistent


than natural reinforcers to which the human brain


is accustomed, they set the stage for addiction.


With time, the brain adapts to the presence of


the drug or alcohol. The removal of the substance


from the host reveals certain abnormalities experienced


by the brain. The host experiences unpleasant


withdrawal symptoms, such as anxiety,


agitation, tremors, increased blood pressure, and in


severe cases, seizures. Naturally, one wants to avoid


painful stimuli; by consuming the substance anew,


ISBN 1-256-72583-8


Chemical Dependency: A Systems Approach, Fourth Edition, by C. Aaron McNeece and Diana M. DiNitto. Published by Pearson. Copyright © 2010 by Pearson Education, Inc.


28 PA R T O N E ● Theories, Models, and Definitions


deprivation are not specific to alcoholism or addiction


to other drugs. In fact, they are commonly


reported by non-addicted adults with a variety of


other psychological problems. Perhaps the most


serious shortcoming is in the psychodynamic theories’


implications for the treatment of alcoholism


or drug addiction. Many counselors warn that a


nondirective approach that focuses solely on the


patients’ development of insight into their problems


neglects the addictive power of alcohol or


other drugs (Cunynghame, 1983).


Nevertheless, there is a feeling among some


scholars (Collins, Blaine, & Leonard, 1999) that


psychodynamic approaches should not be dismissed


because they serve “to guide a substantial


portion of clinical practice” (p. 162). Even though


the empirical support of psychodynamic theory is


scanty, it has shown a remarkable resiliency and the


ability to capture the imagination of practitioners.


Personality Theories. Personality theories,


which frequently overlap the psychodynamic


theories, assume that certain personality traits


predispose an individual to drug use. An individual


with a so-called alcoholic personality is often


described as dependent, immature, and impulsive


(Schuckit, 1986). Other personality theorists


have described alcoholics as highly emotional,


immature in interpersonal relationships, having


low frustration tolerance, being unable to express


anger adequately, and confused in their sex-role


orientation (Catanzaro, 1967). After reviewing


these personality theories, Keller (1972) summarized


them in Keller’s law: The investigation of any


trait in alcoholics will show that they have either


more or less of it. However, the many scales that


have been developed in an attempt to identify alcoholic


personalities have failed to distinguish consistently


the personality traits of alcoholics from


those of non-alcoholics. One of the subscales of


the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory


(MMPI) does differentiate alcoholics from the general


population, but it may actually detect only the


results of years of alcohol abuse, not underlying


personality problems (MacAndrew, 1979).


alcohol can be considered as a point on a continuum


from nonuse, to nonproblem drinking, to


various degrees of deleterious drinking” (p. 191).


Learning theory is helpful in treatment


planning because it addresses the adaptive consequences


of drinking. (For a more extensive discussion


of adaptation and addiction, see Peele


[1998].) Also, behavioral treatments have incorporated


learning theory into a treatment framework


based on the premise that what has been


learned can be unlearned (Bandura, 1969). It


follows that intervening early is important, since


there will be fewer behaviors to unlearn. Learning


theory is also quite adaptable to the systems view,


which is followed throughout this book.


Psychodynamic Theories. Psychodynamic theories


are more difficult to substantiate than most


other psychological theories because they deal with


hard to operationalize concepts and with events that


may have occurred many years before the onset of


addiction. Although Dr. Sigmund Freud never devoted


a single paper to the subject of alcoholism, his


disciples were not the least bit reluctant to apply psychoanalytic


theories to alcohol addiction. The earliest


explanations linked alcoholism with the “primal


addiction” of masturbation (Bonaparte, Freud, &


Kris, 1954). Later, most explanations linked alcoholism


to ego defi ciencies, suggesting that alcohol


is used to attain a sense of security. This theory assumes


that during childhood, inadequate parenting,


along with the child’s individual constitution,


caused the child to form weak attachments to significant


others, resulting in a need to compensate


for or dull the insecurity. This is accomplished in the


consumption of alcoholic beverages (Chordokoff,


1964). Alcohol abuse has also been explained by


psychoanalytic theorists as an expression of hostility


and of homosexuality. Still others view alcoholics


as self-destructive, narcissistic, or orally fi xated


(Schuckit, 1986). Psychoanalytic theory has even


blamed the development of alcoholism on the failure


of mothers to provide milk (Menninger, 1963).


A major problem with psychoanalytic theories


is that experiences such as early childhood


ISBN 1-256-72583-8


Chemical Dependency: A Systems Approach, Fourth Edition, by C. Aaron McNeece and Diana M. DiNitto. Published by Pearson. Copyright © 2010 by Pearson Education, Inc.


C H A P T E R 2 ● The Etiology of Addiction 29


3. A sense of social alienation and a general tolerance


for deviance.


4. A sense of heightened stress. (This may help


explain why adolescence and other stressful


transition periods are often associated with severe


drug and alcohol problems.) (pp. 11, 15).


Research on personality theories of addiction


seems to have waned during the 1990s, and there


are few recent empirical studies that focus on this


explanation for addiction.


Biological Theories


Biophysiological and genetic theories assume


that addicts are constitutionally predisposed to


develop a dependence on alcohol or drugs. These


theories support a medical model of addiction.


Their advocates apply disease terminology and


generally place responsibility for the treatment


of addicts in the hands of physicians, nurses, and


other medical personnel. In reality, the medical


model is generally practiced only during the detoxifi


cation phase.


Generally speaking, biological theories branch


into one of two explanations: neurobiological


and genetic. There has been such an explosion


of knowledge in recent years in the neurobiology


of addiction that we have devoted a separate chapter


to it (see Chapter 3 ). But at this point, we will


briefl y review the research on genetics.


Genetic Theories. Recent studies supported by the


National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA, 2008)


found that a variant in the gene for a nicotinic


receptor subunit doubled the risk for nicotine addiction


among smokers. This is the first evidence


of a genetic variation influencing both the likelihood


of nicotine addiction and an individual’s risk


for the severe health consequences of tobacco use.


National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism


(NIAAA) has funded the Collaborative Studies


on Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA) since 1989, but


specifi c genetic factors have never been established


as a definite cause of alcoholism, although the


There is some evidence that individuals with


an antisocial personality (as defi ned in the DSM-IV ,


APA, 1994) have a higher incidence of alcoholism


than the general population. There is no evidence


that this personality disorder caused the alcoholism,


but these individuals were more disposed to


develop alcohol problems because of their antisocial


personality. Apart from this relatively rare occurrence


of the antisocial personality, alcoholics


have not been found to exhibit a specifi c cluster of


personality traits (Sherfey, 1955). Vaillant (1994)


argues persuasively that personality (as well as psychological)


factors are, at most, of minimal consequence


as a cause of alcoholism. There have been


similar attempts to link a constellation of certain


personality traits to drug addiction as well as alcoholism


(Gossop & Eysenck, 1980). A consensus


seems to have evolved that personality traits are


not of much importance in explaining drug dependence.


In fact, most of those who work in this fi eld


agree that an individual can become dependent


irrespective of personality attributes (Raistrick &


Davidson, 1985). One book lists 94 personality


characteristics that have been attributed to drug


addicts by various theorists (Einstein, 1983).


These include many characteristics that are polar


opposites of one another—for example: poor selfimage


and grandiose self-image, ego infl ation and


ego contraction, self-centered and externalization,


pleasure-seekers and pleasure-avoiders, and several


dozen other contradictory pairs.


A report to the National Academy of Sciences


(“Addictive Personality,” 1983) concludes that


there is no single set of psychological characteristics


that embraces all addictions. However, there


are, according to the report, “signifi cant personality


factors that can contribute to addiction.” These


factors number 4 (not 94) and are as follows:


1. Impulsive behavior, difficulty in delaying


gratifi cation, an antisocial personality, and a


disposition toward sensation seeking.


2. A high value on nonconformity combined


with a weak commitment to the goals for


achievement valued by the society.


ISBN 1-256-72583-8


Chemical Dependency: A Systems Approach, Fourth Edition, by C. Aaron McNeece and Diana M. DiNitto. Published by Pearson. Copyright © 2010 by Pearson Education, Inc.


30 PA R T O N E ● Theories, Models, and Definitions


polymorphisms (Nichols, 1986). A more recent


study reports that the so-called dopamine D2 receptor


gene, which affects the capacity of cells to


absorb dopamine, was present in 77 percent of the


brains of alcoholics and only 28 percent of nonalcoholics


(Blum et al., 1990).


In 1990, the front page of an edition of The


New York Times hailed the discovery of a gene


claimed to be directly linked to alcoholism. Two


years later, this so-called alcoholism gene, formally


known as the dopamine D2 receptor gene, had become


the focus of a bitter controversy. Blum and


Noble insisted that their finding had been amply


documented by subsequent research, and they took


steps to market a test for genetic susceptibility to alcoholism.


Blum suggested that job applicants, children,


and perhaps even fetuses could be tested.


In Blum and Noble’s experiments, the D2


gene was shown to have at least two variants, or


alleles, called A1 and A2. They found the A1 allele


in the genetic material of 69 percent of the alcoholics


studied, compared to only 20 percent of the


controls. Blum and Noble theorized that A1 carriers


may use alcohol or other drugs excessively to


compensate for a reduced ability to absorb pleasureinducing


dopamine.


A study of 862 men and 913 women who


had been adopted early in life by nonrelatives identifi


ed two types of alcoholism (Boham, Cloninger,


von Knorring, & Sigvardsson, 1984). Type I, or


milieu-limited, alcoholism is found in both sexes


and is associated with alcoholism in either biological


parent, but an environmental factor—low


occupational status of the adoptive father—also


had to be present as a condition for alcoholism


to occur in the offspring. Type II, known as malelimited


alcoholism, is more severe but accounts for


fewer cases. It is found only in men, and it does not


appear to be affected by environmental factors.


Vaillant (1983), however, points out the potential


biases in the preceding study. He says that


the study failed to control for the environmental


effect of parental alcoholism. He continues by


pointing out that antisocial personality disorder


must be distinguished from alcohol dependence


statistical associations between genetic factors and


alcohol abuse are very strong (NIAAA, 2009). A


great volume of research has been amassed in this


area over the last several decades, and much of the


evidence points toward alcoholism as an inherited


trait. It has been observed that (1) adopted children


more closely resemble their biological parents


than their adoptive parents in their use of alcohol


(Goodwin, Hill, Powell, & Viamontes, 1973), (2) alcoholism


occurs more frequently in some families


than in others (Cotton, 1979), and (3) concurrent


alcoholism rates are higher in monozygotic twin


pairs (53.5 percent) than in dizygotic pairs (28.3


percent) (Kaij, 1960). Children of alcoholics are


three to seven times more likely to be at risk of alcoholism


(Koopmans & Boomsina, 1995). Having an


alcoholic parent (but not necessarily both parents)


can increase the risk of becoming an alcoholic. Yet


even in the presence of elevated risks, only 33 percent


sons and 15 percent daughters of alcoholics


demonstrate evidence of the disorder.


Some genetic theorists speculate that an


inherited metabolic defect may interact with


environmental elements and eventually lead to


alcoholism. This genetotrophic theory posits an


impaired production of enzymes within the body


(Williams, 1959). Others hypothesize that inherited


genetic traits result in a deficiency of vitamins


(usually of the vitamin B complex), which


leads to a craving for alcohol as well as cellular or


metabolic changes (Tarter & Schneider, 1976).


It is important to remember that, despite the


impressive statistical relationships in these studies


implying a genetic link, no specifi c genetic marker


that predisposes a person toward alcoholism has


ever been isolated. The fi rst biological marker established


for alcoholism was thought to be color


blindness, but a few years later, it was demonstrated


that color blindness was actually a result of


severe alcohol abuse (Valera, Rivera, Mardones, &


Cruz-Coke, 1969). Several other genetic discoveries


have met a similar fate. A workshop on genetic


and biological markers in drug and alcohol abuse


suggests promising areas for genetic research,


such as polymorphisms in gene products and DNA


ISBN 1-256-72583-8


Chemical Dependency: A Systems Approach, Fourth Edition, by C. Aaron McNeece and Diana M. DiNitto. Published by Pearson. Copyright © 2010 by Pearson Education, Inc.


C H A P T E R 2 ● The Etiology of Addiction 31


Genome Project (HGP), supported by the National


Institutes of Health and the U.S. Department


of Energy, has been an important impetus in


the search for genes related to alcohol behavior


(NIAAA, 2000). The research was completed in


2003, but analysis of the data may take several


additional years (HGP, 2008).


Sociocultural Theories


There is little high-quality research regarding the


macrovariables that seek to explain addiction


(Esbensen & Huizinga, 1990). Yet, as we mentioned


earlier, almost every known culture has discovered


the use of beverage alcohol. “All societies establish


a quota of deviance necessary for boundary setting”;


rules around alcohol and drug use are a part


of boundary setting. The ways in which different


societies encourage, permit, or regulate the use of


alcohol varies considerably, however.


For the most part, sociocultural theories have


been generated by observations of differences or


similarities between cultural groups or subgroups.


Sociocultural theorists are prone to attribute differences


in drinking practices, problem drinking, and


alcoholism to environmental factors. For example,


socially disorganized communities often fail to realize


the common values of their residents and to


maintain effective social controls. Therefore, innercity


drug use is more rampant than in the suburbs.


We know that differential rates of alcohol use


between genders vary greatly between nations


(Bloomfi eld, Gmel, & Wilsnack, 2006). Unless greater


biological differences occur between women, from


country to country, or between men, from country


to country (a remote possibility), it seems logical that


culture is a strong infl uence on alcohol use.


According to Goode (1972), the social context


of drug use strongly infl uences, perhaps even


determines, “four central aspects of drug reality”


(p. 3): drug defi nitions, drug effects, drug-related


behavior, and the drug experience. The sociocultural


perspective stands in direct opposition to


what is called the chemicalistic fallacy —the view


that drug A causes behavior X.


and that developmental effects of abusing individuals


must be controlled. Furthermore, for his studies,


Vaillant excludes individuals with other major


psychiatric disorders that could, by themselves,


directly contribute to alcohol dependence. Such


cases (direct and uncomplicated cases) are estimated


to represent 60 to 70 percent of the alcoholdependent


population (Schuckit, 1986).


The notion of Type I and Type II alcoholics


hangs, in part, on the age of the onset of alcoholism.


Vaillant (1983) found in a study of alcohol-abusing


men in inner cities and in college that age of onset


and degree of antisocial symptomatology correlated


with disturbed family environments but was independent


of positive or negative heredity for alcoholism.


In other words, this negated the hypothesis that


heredity predicts the age of onset. “Alcoholic abuse


began 11 years earlier for the socially disadvantaged


men with a heredity negative for alcoholism than for


the college men with two or more alcoholic relatives.”


In other words, early-onset alcohol abusers in inner


cities had no more alcoholic relatives than did lateonset


alcohol abusers in college. Furthermore, innercity


men were 10 times as likely as the college men to


come from multi-problem families, to exhibit traits of


sociopathy, to have delinquent parents, and to have


spent time in jail.


These findings lead one to ask: “How do


biological factors interact with environment to


contribute to heavy enough drinking over long


enough periods of time to produce physical and


psychological dependence?” (Schuckit, 1986).


Vaillant (1983) suggests that rather than there


being two kinds of alcoholism, there may be


(1) genetic loading (predicting whether one develops


alcoholism) and (2) an unstable childhood environment


(predicting when one loses control of


alcohol). (Late onset is less associated with dependence,


substance-related problems, hyperactivity,


and dysfunctional families in one’s youth.)


Genetic research on addiction shows promise,


but it is an incredibly complex activity. Even


the most sanguine recent studies still use phrases


such as “the gene that may infl uence alcoholism


and addiction” (Science Daily, 2007). The Human


ISBN 1-256-72583-8


Chemical Dependency: A Systems Approach, Fourth Edition, by C. Aaron McNeece and Diana M. DiNitto. Published by Pearson. Copyright © 2010 by Pearson Education, Inc.


32 PA R T O N E ● Theories, Models, and Definitions


condones the use of alcohol to relieve those tensions


is likely to have a high rate of alcoholism.


Bales also believed that collective attitudes toward


alcohol use dramatically infl uence rates of alcoholism.


He classified these attitudes as favoring


(1) abstinence, (2) ritual use connected with religious


practices, (3) convivial drinking in a social


setting, and (4) utilitarian drinking (drinking for


personal, self-interested reasons). The utilitarian


attitude, especially in a culture that induces much


inner tension, is the most likely to lead to problem


drinking, whereas the other three mitigate against


alcohol problems.


Also important in Bales’s (1946) theory is the


degree to which a society offers alternatives to alcohol


use for the release of tension and for providing


a substitute means of satisfaction. A social system


with a strong emphasis on upward economic or social


mobility will excessively frustrate an individual


who has no available means of achieving success.


In such a system, high rates of alcohol use would


be expected (Tarter & Schneider, 1976).


Unfortunately, few alternatives to alcohol


or drugs seem to exist in most modern societies.


In traditional societies, such as the hill tribes


of Malaysia, a shaman may assist tribesmen in


achieving a trancelike state in which endorphin


levels are altered (Laderman, 1987). Also at the


supracultural level, Bacon (1974) theorizes that


alcoholism is likely to be a problem in a society that


combines a lack of indulgence of children with


demanding attitudes toward achievement and


negative attitudes regarding dependent behavior in


adults. Another important factor in sociocultural


theories is the degree of societal consensus regarding


alcohol use. In cultures in which there is little


agreement regarding drinking limits and customs,


a higher rate of alcoholism is expected (Trice,


1966). Cultural ambivalence regarding alcohol


use results in weak social controls and allows the


drinker to avoid being labeled as a deviant.


Culture-Specifi c Theories. According to Room,


in “wet” drinking cultures, alcohol is used almost


daily, with few restrictions on availability. Conversely,


Because no object or event has meaning in


the abstract, all these central aspects must be interpreted


in light of social phenomena surrounding


drug use. For example, morphine and heroin


are not very different pharmacologically and biochemically.


Yet heroin is regarded as a dangerous


drug with no therapeutic value, whereas morphine


is defined primarily as a medicine. Definitions


are shaped by the social milieu surrounding


the use of each substance.


People using morphine as an illegal street


drug experience a “rush” or a “high” generally


unknown to patients using the same drug in a


hospital setting. Psychedelic drugs, such as peyote,


which are taken for religious purposes (as in


some Native American churches), do not typically


result in religious or mystical experiences when


taken simply to get high. Drugs, according to


Goode (1972), only potentiate certain kinds of


experiences; they do not produce them. It is important


to distinguish between drug effects and the


drug experience . Many changes may take place in


the body when a chemical is ingested, not all of


which are noted and classifi ed by the user. A drug


may have a more or less automatic effect of dilating


the pupils, causing ataxia or amblyopia, and


so on, but the experience is subject to the cognitive


system of the user’s mind. A person must be


attuned to certain drug effects to interpret them,


categorize them, and place them within appropriate


experiential and conceptual realms (Goode,


1972). One’s propensity to use drugs, the way one


behaves when one uses drugs, and one’s definitions


of abuse and addiction are all influenced by


one’s sociocultural system. Why else would someone


defi ne heroin and LSD as dangerous drugs, yet


almost never perceive social drinkers and smokers


as drug users?


Supracultural Theories. The pioneering work


of Bales (1946) provides some general hypotheses


regarding the relationships among culture, social


organization, and the use of alcohol. He


proposed that a culture that produces guilt, suppressed


aggression, and sexual tension and that


ISBN 1-256-72583-8


Chemical Dependency: A Systems Approach, Fourth Edition, by C. Aaron McNeece and Diana M. DiNitto. Published by Pearson. Copyright © 2010 by Pearson Education, Inc.


C H A P T E R 2 ● The Etiology of Addiction 33


rate is one of the highest in the world, and the Jewish


rate is one of the lowest (Bales, 1946).


Subcultural Theories. There have been many


investigations of sociological and environmental


causes of alcoholism at the subcultural level.


Within the same culture, a great diversity in alcoholism


rates has been related to age, sex, ethnicity,


socioeconomic class, religion, and family background


(Bloomfield, Gmel & Wilsnack, 2006;


Tarter & Schneider, 1976). One of the landmark


studies of social variables at this level was conducted


more than three decades ago by Calahan


(1970). He specifi ed that social environment determines


to a large extent whether an individual


will drink and that sociopsychological variables


also determine the level of drinking. In becoming


a problem drinker, variables such as age, sex, ethnicity,


and social position infl uence the probability


that a person will learn to drink as a dominant


response. Labeling the person as a heavy drinker


then reinforces the probability of that response.


Of course, these processes do not occur in isolation


from other factors, such as the process of


physical addiction. Goode (1984), Laurie (1971),


Imlah (1971), and many others have examined


the sociocultural context of drug addiction and


found there to be many similarities to alcoholism.


A major difference is in the outcast nature of


certain illicit drug users such as heroin addicts.


Users of illegal drugs such as heroin may be more


socially isolated than alcoholics because of their


addiction. Also, certain types of drug addiction


seem to thrive within specifi c subcultures. Heroin


addiction is a persistent problem among jazz musicians.


Inner-city youths frequently “huff ” spray


paint or sniff glue. With three feet of hose and an


empty can, Native American youths on certain


reservations can easily get high on gasoline fumes.


The impact of gender on drug use presents an


interesting perspective on sociocultural theories.


Either a culture-specifi c or subcultural model can


be used in explaining the differences between male


and female drug-related behaviors in the United


States. Historically, female drinking has been less


although legal and social restrictions govern


drinking in “dry” cultures, binge drinking and


even violent drunken behavior may be seen as


acceptable (2001). Levin (1989) describes two


examples of cultural contrast in attitudes toward


drinking: the contrast between French and Italian


drinking practices and the contrast between Irish


and Jewish drinking practices.


There are many similarities between the


French and Italian cultures; both are heavily Catholic


and both produce and consume large quantities


of alcohol. The French, however, drink wine and


spirits, both with meals and without, and both


with and away from the family. The French do


not strongly disapprove of drunkenness, and they


consider it bad manners to refuse a drink. On the


other hand, the Italians drink mostly with meals


and mostly with family, and they usually drink


wine. They strongly disapprove of drunkenness,


and they do not pressure people into drinking. As


one might expect, France has one of the highest


rates of alcoholism in the world, whereas Italy’s


rate is only one-fi fth as great. (In 1952, Italy had


the second-highest rate of wine consumption


in the world, consuming only half of the amount


of wine consumed in France [Kinney & Leaton,


1987].) The strong sanctions against drunkenness


and social control imposed by learning to


drink low-proof alcoholic beverages in moderation


seems to have something to do with the lower


rate of Italian alcoholism.


In a fashion, studies of Irish and Jewish drinking


practices draw some sharp contrasts. The Irish


have high proportions of both abstainers and


problem drinkers, whereas Jews have low proportions


of both (Levin, 1989). The Irish drink largely


outside the home in pubs; Jews drink largely in the


home with the family and on ceremonial occasions.


The Irish excuse drunkenness as “a good man’s


fault”; Jews condemn it as something culturally


alien. Bales found Irish drinking to be largely convivial


on the surface, but purely utilitarian drinking


was a frequent and tolerated pattern. Jewish drinking,


on the other hand, was mostly ceremonial.


Again, it is no surprise that the Irish alcoholism


ISBN 1-256-72583-8


Chemical Dependency: A Systems Approach, Fourth Edition, by C. Aaron McNeece and Diana M. DiNitto. Published by Pearson. Copyright © 2010 by Pearson Education, Inc.


34 PA R T O N E ● Theories, Models, and Definitions


there is no general answer but that the explanation


lies not only in motives but also in a person’s


cultural background, life circumstances, special


life crises, and physical abnormalities. No single


item will be the reason.


Fingarette (1985) believes that it is no harder


for the alcoholic to choose to stop drinking than it is


for others to abandon activities central to their ways


of life. “We should see the alcoholic, not as a sick


and defective human being, but as a human being


whose way of life is self-destructive. The diffi culty we


face is stubborn human nature, not disease” (p. 63).


In a similar fashion, Peele (1988) has examined


the evidence on addiction and concluded


that “we have disarmed ourselves in combating


the precipitous growth of addictions by discounting


the role of values in creating and preventing


addiction and by systematically overlooking the


immorality of addictive misbehavior” (p. 224).


This is not a revival of the addiction as sin model


but an argument that addicts and alcoholics do


differ from other people in the ways in which


they prioritize their values.


As noted at the outset of this chapter, William


S. Burroughs (1977) attempted to answer the


question, “why does a person become a drug


addict?” in his book Junky. “The answer is that


he usually does not intend to become an addict.


You don’t wake up one morning and decide


to be a drug addict . . . You become a narcotics


addict because you do not have strong motivations


in any other direction” (p. xv). Schaler


(2000) has a similar view of addiction. He denies


that there is any such thing as addiction, in


the sense of a “deliberate and conscious course


of action which the person literally cannot stop


doing” (p. xv). He views addiction as a metaphorical


disease, not a physical disease.


Stages of Alcoholism


One of the fi rst attempts to describe the development


of alcoholism is found in Jellinek’s (1952) study of


2,000 male members of Alcoholics Anonymous. He


accepted than male drinking in the United States,


and being intoxicated is clearly more disapproved


of for women than for men (Gomberg, 1986).


Similar gender differences exist throughout the


world (Bloomfi eld, Gmel, & Wilsnack, 2006). These


double standards may account for the much lower


rate of problem drinking noted among women.


Social pressure and social stigma may result in


less problem drinking by women as a subgroup of


the larger U.S. culture.


Some aspects of this phenomenon may be


culture specifi c, however. The fact that men seem


to drink more and have more problems because of


alcohol in some cultures and not in others fi ts into


a supracultural model of drug use. The degree


of female problem drinking appears to be related


to cultural norms regarding the overall status of


women within different societies. Bear in mind


that the vast majority of the research on alcohol


and drug abuse has been conducted on men only.


Only recently have gender-related issues in this


area begun to be systematically examined.


Alternative Explanations


Fingarette (1985) sees alcoholism as “neither sin nor


disease.” Instead, he views it as a lifestyle. According


to Fingarette, proponents of the disease model describe


alcoholism as a disease characterized by loss of


control over drinking. Recovery is possible only if one


voluntarily seeks and enters treatment and voluntarily


abstains from drinking. Only then can one be


cured. Cured from what? From a disease that makes


voluntary abstention impossible and makes drinking


uncontrollable! This, says Fingarette (1985), is an


amazing contradiction.


His alternative explanation views the “persistent


heavy drinking of the alcoholic as a central


activity” of the individual’s way of life. Each person


develops his or her unique way of life, which consists


of a number of central activities. Some will


adopt parenting as a central activity, while others


will place sex, physical thrills, or their careers at


the center. Why do some people choose drinking


as a central feature? Fingarette (1985) says that


ISBN 1-256-72583-8


Chemical Dependency: A Systems Approach, Fourth Edition, by C. Aaron McNeece and Diana M. DiNitto. Published by Pearson. Copyright © 2010 by Pearson Education, Inc.


C H A P T E R 2 ● The Etiology of Addiction 35


(1) all AA members, (2) all in the latter stages of


alcoholism, and (3) all males.


This traditional view of alcohol addiction


was supported by many other prominent scholars,


however. Mann (1968) described alcoholism as a


“progressive disease, which, if left untreated, grows


more virulent year by year” (p. 3). Others seem to


have conveniently ignored available scientifi c evidence


in making assertions such as “the true alcoholic


is no more able to metabolize ethanol than a


diabetic can handle sugar” (Madsen, 1974, p. 94).


Others conclude that alcoholism is the result of an


allergy and that “one does not become an alcoholic:


One is born an alcoholic” (Kessel, 1962, p. 128).

Homework is Completed By:

Writer Writer Name Amount Client Comments & Rating
Instant Homework Helper

ONLINE

Instant Homework Helper

$36

She helped me in last minute in a very reasonable price. She is a lifesaver, I got A+ grade in my homework, I will surely hire her again for my next assignments, Thumbs Up!

Order & Get This Solution Within 3 Hours in $25/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 3 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

Order & Get This Solution Within 6 Hours in $20/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 6 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

Order & Get This Solution Within 12 Hours in $15/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 12 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

6 writers have sent their proposals to do this homework:

Homework Guru
Helping Hand
Top Essay Tutor
University Coursework Help
Writer Writer Name Offer Chat
Homework Guru

ONLINE

Homework Guru

Hi dear, I am ready to do your homework in a reasonable price and in a timely manner.

$62 Chat With Writer
Helping Hand

ONLINE

Helping Hand

Hello, I an ranked top 10 freelancers in academic and contents writing. I can write and updated your personal statement with great quality and free of plagiarism as I am a master writer with 5 years experience in similar ps and research writing projects. Kindly send me more information about your project. You can award me any time as I am ready to start your project curiously. Waiting for your positive response. Thank you!

$55 Chat With Writer
Top Essay Tutor

ONLINE

Top Essay Tutor

I have more than 12 years of experience in managing online classes, exams, and quizzes on different websites like; Connect, McGraw-Hill, and Blackboard. I always provide a guarantee to my clients for their grades.

$65 Chat With Writer
University Coursework Help

ONLINE

University Coursework Help

Hi dear, I am ready to do your homework in a reasonable price.

$62 Chat With Writer

Let our expert academic writers to help you in achieving a+ grades in your homework, assignment, quiz or exam.

Similar Homework Questions

Cirque du soleil competitive advantage - Cisco nexus netflow support - Information Governance Final Research paper - SQL server - How to write a movie plot outline - Unit 6 managing a successful business project assignment sample - Annotated Bibliography - The party line rachel rafelman - Account classification for accounts payable - Nakshatras and their lords - Crafting and executing strategy 22nd edition citation - Pyrex glass tf 55.4 c - BLCN532 Lab 1 Set up your development environment - Week 1 research paper - Self esteem vs self compassion ted talk - Denmark hill to lewisham - Railway board engineering circulars - Managerial Accounting: Assignment 2 - Homework computer - Physical or behavioral signs of incipient puberty - Bendigo health specialist clinics - Costco trade membership vat - Rubber band powered vehicle - Compare and contrast byzantine and romanesque architecture - Australian government ict sustainability plan 2010 2015 - Camshaft position sensor bank 2 - Business Ethics - When will qin shi huang tomb be opened - Air force task list - Apply - Your Career - History short writing assignment - Khan academy solubility rules - Acid indicator crossword clue - Contemporary project management kloppenborg pdf download - Muslim Molvi 7340613399 OnLine No 1 FaMOUs VashIKaraN sPecIaLIsT IN Rajahmundry - Biol 101 quiz 1 - Unit 7Assignment - Final Assessment - Part 1 – Research Paper - Dow jones islamic titans 100 index - Siege james mason hardcover - Vouch recorded sales invoices to supporting shipping documents - Dq response - The taming of the shrew questions and answers - Application of trigonometry in engineering - Derivation of kelvin equation - Conventional representation of an element - Rogerian Argument Essay - ASSIGNMENT 9 - Achieving academic excellence involves - E maketing final - Kelvin hall school hull - Flipkart supply chain case study - Chapter 18 renewable energy review answers - Borehole pump electrical connection - Zlateh the goat plot diagram - What does caesar's will say - Rv malcherek and steel 1981 - Quick analysis totals gallery in excel - Ambridge institute - African history discussion - Module 2= 300 words - The idea that women place a high value on caring and nurturing others is the _____ theory. - Calculate gdp as the value added in production pizzania - Read articles and answer questions - How to train for aesthetic body - Organizational Behavior essay - Calcium carbonate and water reaction equation - Neptune's necklace physiological adaptations - Itil service level management - Nosotros preferimos comprar las entradas tickets para el museo - Baking soda and vinegar limiting reactant lab answers - How to use excel qm linear programming - Area director visit report - Case study of pepsi company pdf - Discussion Topic - Buying proactive at a kiosk - Action Research Report Evaluation 350 Words Total - Lymphoscintigraphy cpt code - The general name for the deepest portion of any basin is - Understanding healthcare management jones and bartlett publishers - The art of public speaking 13th edition pdf free - Pursuit of happyness full movie veoh - Week8 - Smooth criminal riff tab - Deadly unna characters summary - 39 milton street elwood - Nursing assessment. - Negative messages in business - Password Polices and Authentication Methods - Multiracial feminism becky thompson summary - Ife matrix in strategic management - Icheme hazop leader accreditation - The science of snowboarding - Policy legal - American society of public administration code of ethics - Uts meaning in engineering - Brother printer unable to print 4f - Illinois test of academic proficiency - How to get consulting clients fast frank kern pdf - Information taken from an existing classified - One year treasury securities yield percent