Loading...

Messages

Proposals

Stuck in your homework and missing deadline? Get urgent help in $10/Page with 24 hours deadline

Get Urgent Writing Help In Your Essays, Assignments, Homeworks, Dissertation, Thesis Or Coursework & Achieve A+ Grades.

Privacy Guaranteed - 100% Plagiarism Free Writing - Free Turnitin Report - Professional And Experienced Writers - 24/7 Online Support

Purdue pegboard test scoring sheet

30/11/2021 Client: muhammad11 Deadline: 2 Day

JOURNAL OF CHILD AND ADOLESCENT PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY Volume 18, Number 3, 2008 © Mary Ann Liebert, Inc. Pp. 227–236 DOI: 10.1089/cap.2007.0133

Cognitive Effects of Risperidone in Children with Autism and Irritable Behavior

Michael G. Aman, Ph.D.,1 Jill A. Hollway, M.A.,1 Christopher J. McDougle, M.D.,2

Lawrence Scahill, M.S.N., Ph.D.,3 Elaine Tierney, M.D.,4 James T. McCracken, M.D.,5

L. Eugene Arnold, M.D., M.Ed.,1 Benedetto Vitiello, M.D.6 Louise Ritz, M.B.A.,6 Allison Gavaletz, B.S.,3

Pegeen Cronin, Ph.D.,5 Naomi Swiezy, Ph.D.,2 Courtney Wheeler, B.S.,4 Kathleen Koenig, M.S.N.,3

Jaswinder K. Ghuman, M.D.,4 and David J. Posey, M.D.2

Abstract

Objective: The objective of this research was to explore the effects of risperidone on cognitive processes in chil- dren with autism and irritable behavior. Method: Thirty-eight children, ages 5–17 years with autism and severe behavioral disturbance, were randomly assigned to risperidone (0.5 to 3.5 mg/day) or placebo for 8 weeks. This sample of 38 was a subset of 101 sub- jects who participated in the clinical trial; 63 were unable to perform the cognitive tasks. A double-blind placebo- controlled parallel groups design was used. Dependent measures included tests of sustained attention, verbal learning, hand-eye coordination, and spatial memory assessed before, during, and after the 8-week treatment. Changes in performance were compared by repeated measures ANOVA. Results: Twenty-nine boys and 9 girls with autism and severe behavioral disturbance and a mental age �18 months completed the cognitive part of the study. No decline in performance occurred with risperidone. Per- formance on a cancellation task (number of correct detections) and a verbal learning task (word recognition) was better on risperidone than on placebo (without correction for multiplicity). Equivocal improvement also occurred on a spatial memory task. There were no significant differences between treatment conditions on the Purdue Pegboard (hand-eye coordination) task or the Analog Classroom Task (timed math test). Conclusion: Risperidone given to children with autism at doses up to 3.5 mg for up to 8 weeks appears to have no detrimental effect on cognitive performance.

227

Introduction

LITERATURE FOCUSING ON the cognitive effects of atypicalantipsychotics in children and adolescents is exception- ally sparse. The bulk of the literature currently comes from studies of adults with schizophrenia; in addition there are a few investigations with patients having Alzheimer’s disease. Given that antipsychotics often cause sedation, many inves- tigators and clinicians have wondered if cognitive blunting

and/or sedation accompanying early treatment may impair cognition (Ernst et al. 1998; Aman 1984; Aman et al. 1991). The possibility of cognitive impairment seems likely in the short term, as somnolence is a frequent side effect of atypi- cal antipsychotics, especially early in treatment. However, a detailed analysis of adverse events from risperidone treat- ment in children with autism indicated that reports of som- nolence usually dissipated between 2 and 4 weeks after the last dose adjustment of risperidone (Aman et al. 2005). Thus

1Nisonger Center, Ohio State University, Columbus. 2Riley Hospital for Children, Indiana University, Indianapolis. 3Yale Child Study Center, Yale University, New Haven, Conn. 4Kennedy Krieger Institute, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore. 5Department of Psychiatry, Neuropsychiatric Institute, University of California, Los Angeles. 6National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD. This work was funded by contracts from the National Institute of Mental Health (N01MH80011, to Dr. Aman; N01MH70009 to Dr.

Scahill; N01MH70010 to Dr. McCracken; N01MH70001, to Dr. McDougle; M01 RR00750, to Indiana University; M01 RR00052, to Johns Hopkins University, M01 RR 00034, to Ohio State University; and M01 RR06022, to Yale University.

it seems that the time of greatest “risk” of cognitive impair- ment would be in the earliest weeks of treatment.

Literature from adult trials in schizophrenia, have pro- vided us with some insight into the cognitive effects of these drugs. Keefe et al. (1999) conducted a review of 15 studies in which adult patients with schizophrenia were assessed for cognitive effects while taking atypical antipsychotics (AAPs). Contrary to cognitive impairment, they reported improve- ments in attention, executive function, and visuospatial pro- cesses. Purdon (1999) found a handful of studies reporting beneficial effects of AAPs in verbal fluency, verbal learning, and visuomotor tracking. In both reviews, the authors sug- gested that the results of the studies be interpreted with cau- tion, as many studies lacked sufficient controls (e.g., placebo control, double-blind status, and baseline scores). In a few of the studies that did have baseline scores, there was some uncertainty regarding medication status at baseline.

In a more recent review of 26 studies, Stip et al. (2005) an- alyzed the effects of AAPs on cognitive-motor functioning in adults with schizophrenia. This review concluded that a variety of AAPs (risperidone, clozapine, quetiapine) im- proved attention in six studies but showed no change in nine. Please note that summaries often do not total to 26, because not all studies addressed all possible cognitive-motor do- mains and some studies included more than one relevant measure on the domain under discussion. The authors re- ported one study of clozapine in which attention was nega- tively affected. In the area of verbal and working memory, the authors reported that clozapine, olanzapine, quetiapine, and risperidone improved functioning in 12 comparisons while showing no change in 17. Finally, when the authors reviewed the research on motor performance, they found that treatment with the atypicals risperidone, clozapine, and olanzapine was associated with improved motor function- ing in four studies and no change in four others. The find- ings of these adult trials suggest that the atypical antipsy- chotics do not have an adverse effect on cognitive-motor functioning. Indeed, there is some evidence that AAPs may even enhance function.

Recently a few studies have assessed cognitive effects of risperidone in children with severe behavioral disturbance. Günther et al. (2006) assessed open-label risperidone in 23 children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and disruptive behavior disorders (DBDs) and in normal controls matched for age and IQ. No effect of med- ication was found on three tests of sustained and selective attention (continuous performance, divided attention, and Go/No-Go tasks). Troost et al. (2006) tested 24 children with autism spectrum disorders [autism, pervasive developmen- tal disorder not otherwise specified (PDD—NOS), As- perger’s disorder] and found 12 (50%) to be testable on fo- cused attention and divided attention tasks. All participants were shown to be responders to risperidone in a previous clinical trial, and risperidone was withdrawn and gradually replaced with placebo for half of the participants after 24 weeks of treatment. Troost et al. found that performance on the divided attention task (considered to be an index of working memory) deteriorated significantly for subjects re- ceiving placebo substitution in comparison to those main- tained on risperidone.

Although, rarely considered in clinical trials, it has been established that environmental variables can influence the

effects of pharmacotherapy (Yoo et al., 2003). In their study of two doses of risperidone and tangible reinforcement, Yoo et al. (2003), found that risperidone caused a decrease in re- sponse rate and an increase in response time when compared with placebo on a timed visual matching discrimination task. However, these changes were much smaller when tangible reinforcement was added to the risperidone alone condition. The investigators suggested that the added reinforcement had a protective effect on the rate decreasing effects of risperidone.

Pandina et al. (2007) reported on the 6-week acute effects of risperidone and placebo in 228 children with DBDs and subaverage IQ (�84). The cognitive tasks were a continuous performance task (CPT) and a Children’s Verbal Learning Task modified from the California Children’s Verbal Learn- ing Test (Delis et al. 1994). The design was a 6-week, paral- lel-group, comparison of placebo and risperidone (0.02 to 0.06 mg/kg/d). There were no drug-group differences in cognitive functioning in the acute trials.

Finally Aman et al. (in press) assessed 16 children main- tained on risperidone (mainly for DBDs) on a computer-con- trolled cognitive-motor test battery. Subjects were assessed on their regular dose of risperidone and then assigned to one of two drug orders: placebo-risperidone or risperidone- placebo, each of which lasted 2 weeks. Assessments included a Match-To-Sample Memory task, a Short-Term Recognition Memory (STM) test, a Continuous Performance Task (CPT), performance on the Graduated Holes Task (index of static tremor), and seat activity electronically recorded during the memory and CPT tasks. Response time was significantly shorter with risperidone on the STM task, seat activity was significantly lower with risperidone during the STM task, and static tremor was significantly lower with risperidone than placebo on the Graduated Holes Task.

Thus, on balance, most variables failed to show significant changes due to drug condition in these studies with children. However, when they did occur, changes favored risperidone in the Troost et al. (2006) study (working memory) and in the Aman et al. (2007) study (response time, seat activity, sta- tic tremor). The present study was designed to test whether risperidone has an effect on cognitive performance in chil- dren with autism accompanied by serious behavior prob- lems. This was an exploratory comparison, as no data were available with AAPs in children with autism at the time of the trial. The null hypothesis is that there would be no dif- ferences between placebo and risperidone.

Method

Design

This was a multi-site investigation that was conducted at five medical centers. This study used an acute, double blind, placebo controlled, parallel groups design. After being as- sessed at the screen visit, participants who met inclusion cri- teria for the study were then reassessed with clinical instru- ments at baseline and weekly for the next 8 weeks. Matched placebo and risperidone were provided by the manufacturer (Janssen Pharmaceutica) in tablet form. A flexible dosing schedule was based on the participant’s weight, time in the study, clinical response, and emergence of adverse events (see Research Units on Pediatric Psychopharmacology [RUPP] Autism Network, 2002). Cognitive assessments were

AMAN ET AL.228

conducted at baseline, 4 weeks, and 8 weeks. For any par- ticipant who terminated either condition prematurely, the last observation was carried forward to endpoint. Partici- pants’ parents or legal guardians provided written informed consent as approved by institutional review boards of the five participating universities.

Study participants

Participant characteristics, study design, and outcome measures have been described in detail in previous publica- tions (Arnold et al., 2000; RUPP Autism Network, 2002; Sc- ahill et al., 2001). Study participants were male and female children or adolescents, ages 5 to 17 years 2 months, with mental ages �18 months who had autism and severe be- havioral disturbance. To be enrolled in the study, partici- pants received a score of �18 on the Irritability subscale of the Aberrant Behavior Checklist (ABC) (Aman and Singh 1994). In addition, participants must have been rated with a Clinical Global Impressions-Severity (CGI-S) score of �4 by an experienced clinician (CGI-S; NIMH 1985, Arnold et al. 2000), and according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Man- ual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition (DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) a lifetime diagnosis of autistic disorder. The diagnosis of autism was based on a clinical evaluation that included a DSM-IV interview with a parent and direct observation of the participants. The clinical diag- nosis was corroborated by structured interview with one or more parents acting as informants, using the Autism Diag- nostic Interview—Revised (Lord et al. 1994).

Exclusion criteria included positive Beta HCG test for girls; significant medical condition; past history of neu- roleptic malignant syndrome; and, because of dosing con- siderations, weight less than 15 kg. Originally, 52 children and adolescents were randomly assigned to placebo and 49 to risperidone, for a total of 101 study participants. The to- tal number of subjects who had scorable cognitive measures at baseline and at lease one subsequent visit was 38 (37.6%) and, depending on which test was administered, the sample sizes ranged from 8 subjects to 30. Eighteen of the 38 sub- jects were assigned to placebo and 20 to risperidone. The re- maining 63 (62.4%) subjects were not testable either due to the severity of their cognitive impairment and/or disruptive behavior.

Procedures

Any participants receiving psychotropic medicines before the study went through a washout for at least 2 weeks prior to randomization (4 weeks for antipsychotics or fluoxetine). Cognitive assessments were done at Baseline, Week 4 and Week 8. Participants were started at either 0.25 or 0.50 mg with gradual adjustments over the first four weeks. Maxi- mum dose for smaller subjects (15—45 kg) was 2.5 mg/day, whereas the maximum dose for larger participants (�45 kg.) could be as high as 3.5 mg/day.

Outcome measures

The following cognitive measures were obtained.

California Verbal Learning Task–Children’s Version (VLT- C) and Modified VLT-C (MVLT-C) (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 1994). The VLT-C and the MVLT-C measure verbal

memory over brief and intermediate periods of time. Re- spondents were presented with a 10- (MVLT-C) or 15-item (VLT-C) list of nouns on five separate learning trials; most participants were tested with the MVLT-C. The subjects were asked to recall the words in any order after each trial (mea- suring Immediate Free Recall). Once the Trial 5 responses were recorded, the examiner administered the Cancellation Task (see below). This took approximately 10 minutes and prevented subjects from rehearsing the original verbal learn- ing list. Following the Cancellation Task, subjects were asked to recall as many of the words as possible (Long Delay Free Recall). Finally, we administered one Recognition Trial to the respondents, in which previously-presented and new words were used. Subjects then had to determine whether they had heard the word prior to the recognition trial by indicating “yes” or “no” to the examiner.

The dependent measures for the MVLT-C included the to- tal number of words correctly recalled on learning Trials 1- 5 (reflecting verbal memory with exposure and practice), the total number of words correctly recalled after the 10-minute delay (i.e., Long Delay Free Recall), and Recognition Score (by taking the sum of the number of correctly-recognized words and the number of non-list words correctly rejected divided by the total number of words).

The Visuospatial Memory Test (“Dot Test”) (Keefe et al. 1997). The spatial memory test has been shown to be sensi- tive to pharmacological treatment in adults having schizo- phrenia or Alzheimer’s disease. It tests the ability to recall the location of a black dot on a blank sheet of 8.5 by 11 inch paper immediately or following a 10-second delay. The test has 20 trials. Participants were asked to reproduce the posi- tion of the dot on a blank page. Participants sat at a desk in front of a stack of paper that had the numbers 1 to 20 on the back. Trials 1-8 consisted of the no-delay trials and trials 9- 20 were the delayed trials (i.e., 10 seconds). During the de- layed condition, participants were required to name pictures of common objects taken from the Peabody Picture Vocabu- lary Test—Revised (Dunn et al. 1981) to prevent verbal re- hearsal.

Using a metric ruler and a transparent scoring template, and a scoring sheet the examiner measured in millimeters the difference between the stimulus dot and the respondent’s dot on each of the 20 trials. The dependent measures for this protocol included the average no-delay distance, the aver- age 10-second delay distance, and the difference between the two conditions (i.e., averaged delayed recall distance minus the no-delay distance).

Cancellation Task (Barkley, 1991). In this commonly-used test of attention span, the participant was required to scan rows of geometric figures (circles, triangles, stars, diamonds, and squares) and to cancel the target figure, which was a square. All other figures were to be left un-cancelled. Par- ticipants were given 5 minutes in which to cancel 6 pages of stimuli. Correct detections, failures to mark squares (errors of omission), and cancellation of non-targets (errors of com- mission) were recorded as dependent variables.

Purdue Pegboard Task (Tiflin, 1968; Tiflin and Asher, 1948). Our modified version of the original Purdue Peg- board Test was a measure of hand-eye coordination. It re-

RISPERIDONE IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM AND IRRITABLE BEHAVIOR 229

quired the participant to insert as many pins as possible into a column of holes on a wooden pegboard. Each trial lasted 40 seconds. The assessment entailed one practice trial, fol- lowed by three test trials with each hand. The total number of pins correctly inserted was recorded. Study participants alternated between their dominant and non-dominant hands, and the examiner left all pins in place until both the dominant and non-dominant hands had been tested. The ex- aminer then counted and recorded the number of pins in- serted by each hand. Dependent measures for this protocol consisted of the total number of pegs inserted for each hand and the total number of “drops” for each hand.

Classroom Analogue Task (Handen et al., 1990). Prior to giving the Analogue Classroom Task, the child’s math abil- ity was determined by the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT) (Wilkinson, 1993). The WRAT was given only once and was used solely to determine the participant’s math ca- pability and the level at which to conduct testing. The Class- room Analogue Task involved presenting participants with numerous ability-appropriate math problems. These prob- lems ranged from very basic [matching numbers to the size of sets (e.g., finding the correct numeral from 2, 3, 4, 5 when a set of 3 ducks was presented)] to more conventional addi- tion and subtraction. Seven-minute work samples were de- rived from each participant. The task has been extensively used in ADHD research and in work with developmentally disabled patients (Handen et al. 1990), and it has been found to be sensitive to the use of psychostimulant medication (Aman and Pearson, 1999). The dependent measures con- sisted of the total number of math problems that the partic- ipant attempted and total number correctly calculated. The Analogue Classroom Task took 7 minutes to administer.

Consensus rulings on mastery and valid data

Prior to data analysis, the cognitive assessors from each site prepared operational criteria for judging ambiguous re- sponses and for determining whether subjects had mastered the tasks or were simply responding randomly. For exam- ple, on the Cancellation Task we determined that the pencil mark had to actually touch the geometric form in order to be counted (rather than to merely be in its proximity). We also determined that direction of cancellations (left to right and vice versa) did not invalidate performance on this task, although it might influence the count of omission errors (Written consensus operational rules available on request from MGA or JAH.)

Statistical analyses

Analyses were carried out for the children who had valid task measures. Mastery was determined by a set of opera- tional principles applied by the tester at each site, in con- sultation with all other testers, before the blind was broken. The first analyses were t-tests and ChiSquare tests to deter- mine if children with mastery differed from those who did not on the following variables: Age, IQ, Irritability subscale score on the ABC, gender, and CGI-Severity score at base- line. Cognitive data were analyzed as a function of Drug (2 levels; placebo vs. risperidone) and by Time (3 levels, within participants). The General Linear Models package in SPSS was used to analyze the results with repeated measures

ANOVA tests (SPSS, 2003). Effect sizes (partial eta squared, �P

2) were provided in addition to p values. Partial eta squared is the proportion of the effect plus the error vari- ance that is attributable to the effect in the sample. It is not additive as is the squared correlation ratio and according to Keppel and Wickens (2004), it is often the most useful mea- sure of effect for within subject designs. In all tests, p val- ues �.05 were used to indicate statistical significance.

Results

Subjects

Of the 101 participants in the clinical trial of risperidone vs. placebo, 38 (37.6%) were able to provide valid cognitive measures. Of these 38 participants, 12 (31.6%) were risperi- done responders, 8 (21%) were risperidone nonresponders and 18 (47.4%) were randomized to placebo. Of the 63 (62.4%) participants who were unable to complete valid cog- nitive measures, 22 (34.9%) were risperidone responders, 7 (11.1%) were risperidone non-responders and 34 (54%) were randomized to placebo. Characteristics of the 38 participants including functional level, ethnicity, class placement, and parent educational level are presented in Table 1. The modal functional level was mild intellectual disability (n � 18), with average/borderline (n � 8) and moderate intellectual dis- ability (n � 8) also quite common. Ethnically, the large ma- jority was white, and educationally most participants were assigned to special classes. Educational levels of the primary caregivers were quite high, with 19 parents having a uni- versity degree or higher and only one parent without a high school diploma (Table 1). When these participants were com- pared with those who failed to achieve mastery, the follow- ing differences were found: (a) IQ was higher for the Mas- tery group [t (86) � 5.11, p � .001 (M � 62.6 and 39.6, respectively)]; (b) age was higher for the Mastery group [t (99) � 2.05, p � .04 (M � 9.0 and 7.9, respectively)]. No dif- ferences were found for ABC Irritability score, CGI-Severity score, or gender, although Irritability scores and CGI-Sever- ity tended to be nonsignificantly higher (worse) for the non- Mastery group.

Cognitive measures

Table 2 shows the outcome means (and SDs) for Drug by Time on the Cancellation Task, Analogue Classroom Task, and the Verbal Memory Test. The table shows the outcome means (and SDs) and partial eta values (�p2) for Baseline, Week 4 and Week 8, for Time and the Drug by Time inter- action. Analysis of variance indicated two significant effects attributable to Drug (i.e., the interaction of Time and Drug). Risperidone resulted in more correct detections than placebo on the Cancellation Task [F (1, 17) � 3.18, p � .05, �p2 � .16] and more correct recognitions than placebo on the Verbal Learning Task, [F (1, 13) � 4.42, p � .05, �p2 � .20]. Immedi- ate recall on the Verbal Learning Task in both drug condi- tions showed a significant effect of Time (suggesting a sig- nificant practice effect).

Table 3 shows the outcome means (and SDs) for Drug by Time on the Purdue Pegboard Test and the Visuospatial Memory Test. On the Purdue Pegboard, dominant hand in- sertions showed a significant effect over time for both treat- ments (i.e., a practice effect). Results indicated no significant

AMAN ET AL.230

effect attributed to drug (i.e., the Time by Drug interaction). The Visuospatial Task (Dot Test) showed a significant effect of Drug by Time favoring risperidone on the difference score between immediate recall of the dot’s placement (average distance) and the delayed recall of the dot’s placement (av- erage distance–delay), [F (1,6) � 5.22, p � .05, �p2 � .46]. We noted a significant baseline difference (p � .05) by t-test, so we reanalyzed this variable by analysis of covariance (AN- COVA). With this comparison, the interaction term was no longer significant [F (2,5) � 3.29, p � .12].

Discussion

As noted earlier we were able to evaluate the cognitive performance of 38 children with autism. These 38 partici- pants were similar to the remaining 63 (those unable to per- form the cognitive tests) on CGI-Severity score, ABC Irri- tability score, and gender. Not surprisingly, the participants who could be tested had higher IQs (about 23 points, on av- erage), and they were older (9.0 vs. 7.9 years). Thus, there were important differences in presumed cognitive ability and maturity, but they were similar in terms of symptom severity and gender. However, of those assigned to risperi- done, similar proportions of responders and nonresponders could perform the cognitive tests. These findings have im-

plications for generalizability of the findings, a point that we return to later.

No declines, significant or otherwise, were indicated in the measures of attention (i.e., Cancellation Task, timed math test), hand-eye coordination (Purdue Pegboard), or short term verbal memory (verbal learning task). Conversely, sig- nificant improvement occurred with risperidone in two ar- eas of cognitive processing, namely on the Cancellation Task (correct detections) and on the Verbal Learning Task (correct recognitions); improvement occurred equivocally on the Spatial Memory Task (Dot Test, difference score). Whereas this stability of cognitive performance may appear to be at odds with reported tiredness with risperidone (Aman et al., 2005), the two are not necessarily inconsistent. For example risperidone caused a large reduction in irritable/disruptive behavior (E.S. � 1.20) and hyperactivity (E.S. � 1.00), which may have enabled the participants to perform as well or bet- ter, even if somnolence were present.

Purdon’s (1999) review of atypical antipsychotic studies in adults with schizophrenia suggested enhancement in the areas of verbal fluency and attention, although he cautioned that some investigations used open-label designs. In our study, subjects taking risperidone showed significant im- provement compared with controls on verbal learning recog- nition performance but not on short-term or delayed recall.

RISPERIDONE IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM AND IRRITABLE BEHAVIOR 231

TABLE 1. SAMPLE CHARACTERISTICS (N � 38)*

Age, yr, mean � SD (range) 9.42 � 2.96 (5–17 yrs)

Sex, no. (%) Male 29 (76.3) Female 9 (23.7)

Race, no. (%) White 31 (81.57) Black 2 (5.26) Asian/Pacific Islander 3 (7.89) Hispanic 2 (5.26)

Functional level, no. (%)** Average/low average ability 8 (21.05) Mild intellectual disability 18 (47.36) Moderate intellectual disability 8 (21.05) Severe intellectual disability 1 (2.63) Profound intellectual disability 2 (5.26)

Daily dose risperidone (mg), mean � SD*** 1.12 � 0.44

Class placement, no. (%) Regular school class 4 (10.53) Special class 30 (78.94) MRDD school 4 (10.53)

Primary caregiver education level, no. (%) Graduate degree 8 (21.05) University degree 11 (28.95) Some college 7 (18.42) Trade school 3 (7.89) High school 8 (21.05) �High School 1 (2.63)

*There were 63 children who could not be successfully assessed. Testable participants had a signifi- cantly higher IQ (23 points higher) and higher mean age (9.0 vs. 7.9 years) than the untestable subjects.

**IQ score unavailable for one subject. ***Range = 0.5–2.0 mg/day.

T A

B L

E 2.

F IN

D IN

G S

F O

R C

A N

C E

L L

A T

IO N

, A

N A

L O

G U

E C

L A

S S

R O

O M

, A

N D

V E

R B

A L

L E

A R

N IN

G T

A S

K S

P la

ce bo

R is

pe ri

do n

e M

ea n

( S

D )

M ea

n (

S D

) A

N O

V A

R es

u lt

s

F �

P 2

F D

ru g

x D

ru g

x C

og n

it iv

e A

ss es

sm en

ts B

as el

in e

W ee

k 4

W ee

k 8

B as

el in

e W

ee k

4 W

ee k

8 T

im e

T im

e T

im e

C an

ce ll

at io

n T

as k

C o

rr ec

t D

et ec

ti o

n s

11 0.

71 (

56 .3

)a 76

.4 3

(6 3.

9) 95

.2 9

(5 9.

7) 12

6. 75

( 46

.9 )b

13 1.

33 (

40 .3

) 13

8. 50

( 59

.7 )

2. 10

3. 18

* .1

60 C

o m

m is

si o

n s

2. 29

( 4.

3) 5.

29 (

5. 6)

4. 09

( 9.

7) 6.

50 (

22 .2

) 0.

00 (

0. 0)

0. 08

( 0.

3) .2

3 1.

03 .0

60 O

m is

si o

n s

19 .0

0 (2

0. 3)

29 .5

7 (3

5. 5)

27 .5

7 (3

3. 8)

11 .7

5 (2

2. 6)

5. 83

( 9.

8) 10

.0 0

(1 9.

3) .2

2 1.

24 .0

70

A n

al o

g u

e C

la ss

ro o

m T

as k

N u

m b

er A

tt em

p te

d 31

.6 3

(1 1.

0) c

29 .2

5 (1

3. 6)

32 .5

0 (1

5. 7)

22 .8

8 (1

4. 7)

d 19

.6 3

(9 .3

) 24

.5 0

(5 .8

) .7

4 .0

3 .0

00 N

u m

b er

C o

rr ec

t 24

.5 0

(1 0.

5) 21

.3 8

(1 3.

8) 26

.6 3

(1 8.

0) 19

.2 5

(1 1.

4) 17

.7 5

(8 .8

) 20

.5 0

(6 .6

) .8

1 .0

8 .0

10

V er

b al

L ea

rn in

g T

as k

Im m

ed ia

te R

ec al

l 25

.0 0

(8 .8

)e 28

.0 0

(1 1.

4) 29

.8 8

(8 .9

)0 28

.5 0

(1 1.

9) f

31 .9

2 (1

2. 4)

31 .9

2 (1

1. 6)

5. 48

** .2

8 .0

15 D

el ay

ed R

ec al

l 4.

67 (

2. 9)

g 6.

33 (

8. 4)

4. 83

( 2.

9) 6.

20 (

4. 6)

h 4.

80 (

3. 8)

7. 40

( 2.

7) .1

5 1.

31 .0

90 R

ec o

g n

it io

n 81

.0 0

(1 1.

6) i

66 .3

3 (9

.0 )0

75 .0

0 (1

4. 0)

77 .8

9 (1

9. 1)

j 85

.2 3

(1 6.

5) 83

.1 7

(1 5.

4) .6

0 4.

42 *

.2 50

Sa m

p le

s iz

es d

if fe

r b

ec au

se m

an y

o f

th e

p ar

ti ci

p an

ts c

o u

ld n

o t

d o

a ll

c o

g n

it iv

e ta

sk s

an d

, i n

s o

m e

ca se

s, s

u b

je ct

s co

u ld

n o

t p

er fo

rm a

ll p

ar ts

o f

th e

g iv

en t

as k

. a n

� 7.

b n

� 12

. c n

� 8.

d n

� 8.

e n

� 8.

f n

� 12

. g n

� 6.

h n

� 10

. i n

� 5.

j n �

10 .

* p

� 0.

05 , *

* p

� 0.

01 .

T A

B L

E 3.

F IN

D IN

G S

F O

R P

U R

D U

E P

E G

B O

A R

D A

N D

SP A

T IA

L M

E M

O R

Y T

A S

K S

P la

ce bo

R is

pe ri

do n

e M

ea n

( S

D )

M ea

n (

S D

) A

N O

V A

R es

u lt

s

F �

P 2

F D

ru g

x D

ru g

x C

og n

it iv

e A

ss es

sm en

ts B

as el

in e

W ee

k 4

W ee

k 8

B as

el in

e W

ee k

4 W

ee k

8 T

im e

T im

e T

im e

P u

rd u

e P

eg b

o ar

d D

o m

in an

t h

an d

i n

se rt

24 .0

8 (1

0. 5)

a 27

.6 1

(1 3.

0) 28

.3 8

(1 3.

9) 32

.7 6

(1 7.

4) b

32 .8

2 (1

9. 2)

34 .5

9 (1

6. 0)

3. 13

* 1.

06 .0

4 N

o n

d o

m in

an t

h an

d i

n se

rt 22

.3 8

(9 .9

) 26

.0 0

(7 .3

) 24

.2 3

(1 0.

3) 26

.8 2

(1 8.

4) 29

.2 3

(1 7.

4) 29

.5 3

(1 4.

7) .1

4 .8

0 .0

1 D

o m

in an

t h

an d

d ro

p s

2. 77

( 2.

4) 3.

08 (

2. 3)

3. 00

( 2.

6) 2.

35 (

1. 9)

3. 76

( 3.

4) 4.

00 (

3. 4)

1. 86

.9 4

.0 3

N o

n d

o m

in an

t h

an d

d ro

p s

2. 31

( 2.

4) 3.

77 (

3. 2)

3. 54

( 2.

9) 3.

24 (

3. 0)

2. 59

( 2.

0) 4.

00 (

3. 0)

1. 25

1. 53

.0 5

B o

th h

an d

s in

se rt

s 46

.4 6

(1 9.

5) 51

.6 2

(1 9.

2) 52

.6 2

(2 3.

2) 59

.5 9

(3 5.

3) 62

.0 6

(3 6.

1) 64

.1 2

(3 0.

0) 2.

91 .4

7 .0

2 B

o th

h an

d s

d ro

p s

5. 08

( 4.

3) 6.

85 (

4. 9)

6. 54

( 4.

9) 5.

59 (

4. 0)

6. 35

( 4.

8) 8.

00 (

5. 5)

2. 10

.5 2

.0 2

D o

t T

es t

A v

er ag

e D

is ta

n ce

3. 52

( 1.

8) c

2. 47

( .7

9) 2.

90 (

1. 3)

2. 40

( 1.

0) d

2. 75

( .9

6) 2.

57 (

1. 3)

.7 4

2. 75

.3 1

A v

er ag

e D

is ta

n ce

- D

el ay

4. 95

( 2.

2) 6.

12 (

2. 8)

5. 27

( 2.

6) 7.

19 (

2. 34

) 5.

19 (

3. 0)

5. 08

( 2.

4) .6

4 2.

28 .2

7 D

if fe

re n

ce 1.

43 (

1. 5)

3. 65

( 2.

4) 2.

38 (

2. 9)

4. 79

( 2.

0) 2.

44 (

2. 2)

2. 52

( 2.

0) .5

1 5.

22 *

.4 6

Sa m

p le

s iz

es d

if fe

r b

ec au

se m

an y

o f

th e

p ar

ti ci

p an

ts c

o u

ld n

o t

d o

a ll

c o

g n

it iv

e ta

sk s.

a n

� 13

. b n

� 17

. c n

� 4.

d n

� 4.

* p

� 0.

05 .

Current literature is mixed regarding the effect of risperi- done on spatial memory. Results from Reilly, Harris, Ke- shaven and Sweeney (2006), in subjects with schizophrenia, showed worsening of deficits in the maintenance of spatial working memory. In contrast, a comparison of clozapine and risperidone in patients with schizophrenia conducted by McGurk et al. (2005) found improvements in spatial mem- ory with risperidone and worsening with clozapine. Our study suggests an equivocal improvement in delayed spa- tial memory for the risperidone group compared to placebo controls. (Loss of significance with the ANCOVA may have been due merely to reduced degrees of freedom, given the smallness of the sample.)The “significant” finding was based on a difference score between no-delay and delay conditions, and only 8 participants could conduct the task. As the find- ing of improvement with risperidone can result from wors- ening in the no-delay or improvement in the delay condition with risperidone (or the opposite with placebo), this “sig- nificant” outcome needs replication.

The choice of cognitive-motor tasks in children with de- velopmental disabilities has varied greatly, and results have also differed across cognitive constructs. Results of the can- cellation task used in this study suggest that the risperidone- treated participants were more task oriented than placebo- treated subjects. This is not consistent with the Günther et al. (2006) study, which found no risperidone effects on at- tention in adolescents with ADHD. However, in Troost et al. (2006) and in the present study, aspects of memory and at- tention did improve (Troost et al., characterized their divided attention task as an index of working memory.) Findings for the Verbal Learning test differed from the Pandina (2007) study in that we observed enhanced word recognition with risperidone, but Pandina et al. did not. (However, Pandina et al. did report improvements in uncontrolled one year open-label extension studies.) We are not aware of any other study with children having pervasive developmental disor- ders in which the Visuospatial Memory Task has been used. This is not surprising in that it proved to be very difficult to convey instructions to these children on how to perform the Dot Test.

Children with autism and accompanying irritable behav- ior are exceptionally difficult to assess for cognitive change. Troost et al. (2006) commented that a testable rate of 50% of children with PDDs was “very favorable” and “would have been lower if a larger proportion of the children . . . had an autistic disorder.” (pg. 571) Indeed, only 2 of 14 participants (14.3%) in the Troost et al. study had a diagnosis of autism, whereas the remainder had PDD—NOS or Asperger’s dis- order. Our study was conducted exclusively with children having autistic disorder accompanied by serious behavior problems. Furthermore, the large majority of the original sample (83%) had intellectual disability. Collectively, these features made this group of children very challenging to test. As such, our data may provide important information on the selection of cognitive-motor tasks in this population. The Purdue Peg Board task was clearly the easiest for these youngsters to understand. By contrast, the “Dot Test” was remarkably difficult for them to grasp. The MVLT-C, Can- cellation Task, and Analogue Classroom Task were of inter- mediate difficulty. It is easy to see how all of these latter tests may reflect important skills that are relevant to learning in traditional classroom settings. The advantages of being able

to sustain attention better over time (as in the Cancellation task) and to be more capable of recognizing previously-heard words would seem apparent both in academic settings and in real life. Likewise, enhanced memory for location (Visu- ospatial Task) would seem to carry advantages in real-life settings as well. As partial eta squared ranged from .16 to .46, this indicates that the drug condition actually accounted for substantial amounts of variance in these outcome vari- ables. Nevertheless, it is also somewhat reassuring that there were no indications of risperidone-associated deterioration on any of the other variables.

Limitations

This study has several limitations that caution against over-embracing the statistically-significant (and one equivo- cal) findings. First, given the exploratory nature of this work (there is only one other study of atypical antipsychotics in children with PDDs), we adopted the .05 level for alpha. Had we corrected for multiple comparisons, none of the compar- isons would have exceeded alpha. Second, only a minority of our participants were able to perform these tasks. This re- sulted in (a) small sample sizes and (b) the observation that the testable group had a higher IQ and was older than the untestable group. This indicates that the findings may not be fully representative of what would be observed if all the 101 clinical trial participants were testable. However, as re- ported by others (Troost et al. 2006), children with autism and irritable behavior are very challenging to assess with such cognitive tests, and the proportion of participants as- sessed was probably quite respectable under the circum- stances. Third, we have already stated our reservations about the “significant” drug finding for the Dot test in that only a small number of participants could perform it.

Despite these obvious limitations, the findings are note- worthy for several reasons. First, autistic disorder is often coupled with substantial cognitive disability. Therefore, it is important that pharmacotherapy not increase any functional handicap that is already present. We did not see evidence of risperidone-induced deficits in performance, and there was some indication of enhancement on some variables. Second, the magnitude of change, assessed by partial eta squared, suggests sizable gains in adaptive skills if upheld by future studies. Third, it is worth noting that the data were gathered across multiple sites under double-blind conditions, which may help to discount any individual examiner effects (i.e., unintended bias). Finally, the mechanism of any improve- ment is unknown. These were performance tests which tapped what the participant was able to achieve on test days. At this stage, it is unknown whether the participants were simply more compliant (i.e., the changes were a secondary consequence of suppressed irritable behavior) or whether risperidone affected true cognitive ability at a more basic level.

Clinical implications

The results reported here are reassuring to clinicians pre- scribing risperidone for school children with autistic disor- der. It does not appear to impair academic ability or cogni- tive-motor performance. However, at the current state of knowledge, clinicians should not conclude that risperidone can be counted on to improve cognitive-motor performance

AMAN ET AL.234

or even that it is innocuous. The main value of these find- ings is to reduce one, and only one, of several safety con- cerns, with the serious possibilities of metabolic and neuro- logical risks remaining. Further research will be needed to determine the robustness and extent of any favorable effects on cognitive performance.

Disclosures

Dr. Aman has affiliations with Bristol-Myers Squibb Co., Forest Research Institute, Johnson and Johnson, Neu- ropharm, and Supernus. Dr. Arnold has affiliations with Shire, Neuropharm, Lilly, Novartis, Organon, Janssen, and McNeil. Dr. Ghuman has affiliations with Bristol-Myers Squibb Co. Dr. McDougle has affiliations with Bristol-Myers Squibb Co., Eli Lilly and Co., Forest Research Institute, Janssen Pharmaceutica, and McNeil Pediatrics. Dr. Scahill has affiliations with Janssen Pharmceutica, Bristol-Myers Squibb Co., Neuropharm and Supernus. Dr. Posey has affil- iations with Bristol-Myers Squibb Co., Eli Lilly and Co., For- est Research Institute, and Shire. Dr McCracken has afflia- tions with Bristol Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, McNeil Pediatrics, Janssen Pharmaceutica, Pfizer, Shire, UCB, Wyeth, and No- vartis. Dr. P. Cronin, Ms. A. Gavaletz, Ms. J. Hollway, Ms. K. Koenig, Ms. L. Ritz, Dr. Swiezy, Dr. Tierney, Dr. Vitiello, and Ms. Wheeler have no affiliations.

The opinion and assertions contained in this report are the private views of the authors and are not to be construed as official or as reflecting the views of the Department of Health and Human Services, the National Institutes of Health or the National Institutes of Mental Health.

References

Aman, MG: Drugs and learning in mentally retarded persons. In G. D. Burrows & J. S. Werry (Eds.), Advances in Human Psychopharmacology (Vol. 3, pp.121–163). Greenwich, Ct.: JAI Press, 1984.

Aman MG, Arnold L, McDougle CJ, Vitiello B, Scahill L, Davies M, McCracken JT, Tierney E, Nash PL, Posey DJ, Chuang S, Martin A, Shah B, Gonzalez NM, Swiezy NB, Ritz L, Koenig K, McGough J, Ghuman JK, Lindsay RL: Acute and long-term safety and tolerability of risperidone in children with autism. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 15:869–884, 2005.

Aman MG, Hollway JA, Leone S, Masty J, Lindsay R Nash P, Arnold LE: Effects of risperidone on cognitive-motor perfor- mance and motor movements in chronically medicated chil- dren. Res Dev Disabil. (in press).

Aman MG, Marks RE, Turbott SH, Wilsher CP, Werry SN: Methylphenidate and thioridazine in the treatment of intel- lectually subaverage children: Effects on cognitive-motor per- formance. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 30:816–824, 1990.

Aman MG, Pearson DA: Monitoring and measuring drug ef- fects. II. Behavioral, emotional, and cognitive effects. In JS Werry, MG Aman (eds.) Practitioner’s Guide to Psychoactive Drugs for Children and Adolescents (2nd ed). New York: Plenum Press, 1999. pp. 99–164.

Aman MG, Singh NN: Aberrant Behavior Checklist Manual. East Aurora, NY: Slosson Educational Publications, 1994.

Arnold LE, Aman MG, Martin A, Collier-Crespin A, Vitiello B, Tierney E, Asarnow R, Bell-Bradshaw F, Freeman BJ, Gates- Ulanet P, Klin A, McCracken JT, McDougle CJ, McGough JJ, Posey DJ, Scahill L, Swiezy NB, Ritz L, Volkmar F: Assess-

ment in multisite randomized clinical trials of patients with autistic disorder: The Autism RUPP Network. J Autism Dev Disord 30:99–111, 2000.

American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association, 1994.

Bilder RM, Goldman RS, Volavka J, Czobor P, Hoptman M, Sheitman B, Lindenmayer J, Citrome L, McEnvoy J, Kunz M, Chakos M, Cooper TB, Horowitz TL, Lieberman JA: Neu- rocognitive effects of clozapine, olanzapine, risperidone, and haloperidol in patients with chronic schizophrenia or schizoaf- fective disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 159:1018–1028, 2002.

Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA: California Verbal Learning Test Manual—Children’s Version. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation, 1994.

Dunn LM, Dunn LM, Robertson GJ, Eisenberg JL: Peabody Pic- ture Vocabulary Test—-Revised (PPVT-R). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service, 1981.

Ernst M, Malone RP, Rowan AB, George R, Gonzalez NM, Silva RR: Antipsychotics (neuroleptics). In JS Werry, MG Aman (eds.). Practitioner’s Guide to Psychoactive Drugs for Children and Adolescents. New York: Plenum, 1988. Pp. 297–327.

Günther T, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Jolles J, Konrad K: The in- fluence of risperidone on attentional functions in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and co-morbid disruptive behavior disorder. J Child Adolesc Psy- chopharmacol. 16:725–735, 2006.

Handen BL, Broaux AM, Gosling A, Ploof DL, Feldman H: The efficacy of methylphenidate among mentally retarded chil- dren with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 86: 922–930, 1990.

Keefe RS, Silva SG, Perkins DO, Lieberman JA: The effects of atypical antipsychotic drugs on neurocognitive impairment in schizophrenia: A review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull. 25:201–222, 1999.

Keefe RS, Lees-Roitman SE, Dupre RL: Performance of patients with schizophrenia on a pen and paper visuospatial working memory task with short delay. Schizophr Res. 26:9–14, 1997.

Keppel G, Wickens TD: Design and Analysis: A Researcher’s Handbook (4th. ed.). New Jersey: Prentice Hall, 2004.

Lord C, Rutter M. LeCouteur A: Autism diagnostic interview- revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for care- givers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord: 24:659–685, 2004.

McGurk SR, Carter CC, Goldman R, Green MF, Marder SR, Haiyi X, Schooler NR, Kane JM: The effects of clozapine and risperidone on spatial working memory in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 162:1013–1016, 2005.

National Institute of Mental Health: CGI (Clinical Global Im- pressions Scale). Psychopharmacol Bull. 21:839–844, 1985.

Pandina GJ, Bilder R, Harvey PD, Keefe RSE, Aman MG, Gharab- awi G: Risperidone and cognitive function in children with dis- ruptive behavior disorders. Biol Psychiatry, 62, 226–234, 2007.

Purdon SE. Cognitive improvement in schizophrenia with novel antipsychotic medications. Schizophr Res. 35:S51–S60, 1999.

Reilly JL, Harris MS, Keshavan MS, Sweeney JA: Adverse effects of risperidone on spatial working memory in first-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 63:1189–1197, 2006.

Research Units on Pediatric Psychopharmacology Autism Net- work: Risperidone in children with autism and serious be- havioral problems. N Engl J Med. 347:314–321, 2002.

Scahill L, McCracken J, McDougle CJ, Aman MG, Arnold LE, Tierney E, Cronin P, Davies M, Ghuman J, Gonzalez N, Koenig K, Lindsay R, Martin A, McGough J, Posey DJ, Swiezy N, Volkmar F, Ritz L, Vitiello B: Methodological issues in de-

RISPERIDONE IN CHILDREN WITH AUTISM AND IRRITABLE BEHAVIOR 235

signing a multisite trial of risperidone in children and ado- lescents with autism. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 11: 377–388, 2001.

SPSS: SPSS 13.0 for Windows. Chicago: SPSS, 2003. Stip E, Chouinard S, Boulay LJ: On the trail of a cognitive en-

hancer for the treatment of schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsy- chopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 29:219–232, 2005.

Tiflin J, Asher EJ: The Purdue Pegboard: Norms and studies of reliability and validity. J Appl Psychology. 32:234–247, 1948.

Tiflin J: Purdue Pegboard Examiner Manual. Chicago: Science Research Associates, 1968.

Troost PW, Althaus M, Lahius BE, Buitelaar JK, Minderaa RB, Hoekstra PJ: Neuropsychological effects of risperidone in chil- dren with pervasive developmental disorders: A blinded dis- continuation study. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 16:561–573, 2006.

Wilkinson GS: Wide Range Achievement Test—Third Edition. Administration Manual. Wilmington DE: Wide Range, Inc, 1993.

Yoo HJ, Williams DC, Napolitano DA, Peyton RT, Baer DM, Schroeder SR: Rate-decreasing effects of the atypical neu- roleptic risperidone attenuated by conditions of reinforcement in a woman with mental retardation. J Appl Behav Anal. 36:245–248, 2003.

Address reprint requests to: Jill A. Hollway, M.A.

The Nisonger Center, UAP 1581 Dodd Drive

Homework is Completed By:

Writer Writer Name Amount Client Comments & Rating
Instant Homework Helper

ONLINE

Instant Homework Helper

$36

She helped me in last minute in a very reasonable price. She is a lifesaver, I got A+ grade in my homework, I will surely hire her again for my next assignments, Thumbs Up!

Order & Get This Solution Within 3 Hours in $25/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 3 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

Order & Get This Solution Within 6 Hours in $20/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 6 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

Order & Get This Solution Within 12 Hours in $15/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 12 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

6 writers have sent their proposals to do this homework:

A Grade Exams
High Quality Assignments
Assignment Solver
Top Grade Tutor
24/7 Assignment Help
Top Quality Assignments
Writer Writer Name Offer Chat
A Grade Exams

ONLINE

A Grade Exams

I am a PhD writer with 10 years of experience. I will be delivering high-quality, plagiarism-free work to you in the minimum amount of time. Waiting for your message.

$18 Chat With Writer
High Quality Assignments

ONLINE

High Quality Assignments

I reckon that I can perfectly carry this project for you! I am a research writer and have been writing academic papers, business reports, plans, literature review, reports and others for the past 1 decade.

$38 Chat With Writer
Assignment Solver

ONLINE

Assignment Solver

I have read your project details and I can provide you QUALITY WORK within your given timeline and budget.

$33 Chat With Writer
Top Grade Tutor

ONLINE

Top Grade Tutor

I am a PhD writer with 10 years of experience. I will be delivering high-quality, plagiarism-free work to you in the minimum amount of time. Waiting for your message.

$40 Chat With Writer
24/7 Assignment Help

ONLINE

24/7 Assignment Help

I am an experienced researcher here with master education. After reading your posting, I feel, you need an expert research writer to complete your project.Thank You

$23 Chat With Writer
Top Quality Assignments

ONLINE

Top Quality Assignments

Being a Ph.D. in the Business field, I have been doing academic writing for the past 7 years and have a good command over writing research papers, essay, dissertations and all kinds of academic writing and proofreading.

$38 Chat With Writer

Let our expert academic writers to help you in achieving a+ grades in your homework, assignment, quiz or exam.

Similar Homework Questions

The unwanted walter wykes - 2 iodopropane sodium hydroxide - 10 best mathematical mnemonics - What is cephalocaudal and proximodistal development - Cultures that tend to support good strategy execution include - Mole fraction and partial pressure - Learning in psychology ppt - Charles sturt university dental clinic dubbo - Ideal citizen in a totalitarian government - Why were the georgia guidestones built - Target customer of milk tea - You will perform research and appraisal of existing evidence related to challenges to the safe and effective care of the aging population in the context of global health. - Altium via in pad - Measuring the three components of shyness - Philip markoff girlfriend - Summarize content in textbook - Icrs cartilage grading system - Assignment: Professional Leadership Journal: Narcissism Assessment - Math - Cole national group - Classroom management reflection paper - Barbirolli square manchester parking - Internet of things chapter 1 quiz - Soil improvement techniques ppt - Kiss with a fist karaoke - A voltaic cell is constructed with an ag ag+ - Paul williams temptations drunk on stage - Balancing nuclear equations practice - 16 union street tighes hill - Michael thomas web developer - Patterns in a literary work are rarely - Chord me and my broken heart - Tibco enterprise message service - Area of sector and segment - Bucket map join in hive - How to draw price elasticity of demand curve in excel - Azure site recovery architecture ppt - Depth of field refers to - Environmental physics clare smith pdf - Chem 121 predicting products of chemical reactions - Sizzling starts for persuasive writing - Terror's purse strings annotated - 3/32 arundel street fremantle - Security advantages of cloud-based solutions. - New lithographic equipment acquired at a cost of - What does oedipus promise to do - Writing in the moment - Communication - Eat drink man woman netflix - Atom company just began business - Advantages and disadvantages of delayering - Abercrombie and fitch ceo quote - Fallout 76 unsolved tracking terror clues - Bsbcmm401 make a presentation ppt - George frisbie hoar on the chinese exclusion act answers - Can i paint over bitumen paint - Tortillas by jose antonio burciaga thesis - What does aco in healthcare mean - Epilogue - Little red midnight remember - Plot diagram template doc - Powers powder actuated tool - Genesis 19 15 26 - NEED 3+ PAGES WITH 4 Peer reviewed REFERENCES CITED IN APA FORMAT - Site and situation of new york - IS THE U.S. HEALTHCARE SYSTEM SICK? - Assignment Due 10/29/2020 APA Format - Melting point of urea and cinnamic acid - Weight Management - Final Project Paper - Which scenario might indicate a reportable insider threat security incident - What is a lead digit - Assignment: Social History - Types of experimental errors - Anthropology exam - Write an eight sentence paragraph that fully develops the topic - Strategy, Planning, and Selection - Case mix index calculation - SPCH 324 FINAL ASSESSMENT - Ubc 1997 building code volume 2 - The standover man quotes - Organizing function of management ppt - Mrcp part 1 registration - Voltage controlled crystal oscillator - Community - Lissajous figures frequency ratio - Driscoll model of reflection pdf - St francis church maidstone - How to tighten swarovski bracelet - Quality in qualitative evaluation a framework for assessing research evidence - Discussion needed by 3pm Sat - Identify the element in qa3 physical environment - Ju - Rph radio reading network - World Geography - DUE IN A 1.5 HOURS! - Discussion 1 – Setting the Stage for Success - Amada punching machine programming - 540 fe - Conductor meaning in science - Persuasive Speech