Loading...

Messages

Proposals

Stuck in your homework and missing deadline? Get urgent help in $10/Page with 24 hours deadline

Get Urgent Writing Help In Your Essays, Assignments, Homeworks, Dissertation, Thesis Or Coursework & Achieve A+ Grades.

Privacy Guaranteed - 100% Plagiarism Free Writing - Free Turnitin Report - Professional And Experienced Writers - 24/7 Online Support

Social psychology 9th edition pdf aronson

20/10/2021 Client: muhammad11 Deadline: 2 Day

Social Psychology

. First Paper: You will be assigned one 3-page paper, in which you willapply a concept or principle within the field of social psychology to discuss a scene from a movie, television show, or novel. You should include a clear definition of the concept,explain how the concept is illustrated by the fictionalscene,and conclude with a testable hypothesis for a social psychologicalexperiment related to the concept. the movie will be jim carrey liar liar

Social Psychology

Elliot Aronson

University of California, Santa Cruz

Timothy D. Wilson

University of Virginia

Robin M. Akert

Wellesley College

Slides prepared by
Travis Langley, Henderson State University

(A few additional slides prepared by

Weylin Sternglanz and Amanda Mahaffey,

Nova Southeastern University)

7th edition

*

Chapter 4

Social Perception: How We Come to Understand Other People

“Things are seldom as they seem. Skim milk masquerades as cream.”

– W. S. Gilbert

*

Other people are not easy to figure out.

Why are they the way they are?

Why do they do what they do?

We all have a fundamental fascination with explaining other people’s behavior, but all we have to go on is observable behavior:

What people do
What they say
Facial expressions
Gestures
Tone of voice
We can’t know, truly and completely, who they are and what they mean.

Instead, we rely on our impressions and personal theories, putting them together as well as we can, hoping they will lead to reasonably accurate and useful conclusions.

*

This basic aspect of human cognition has been exploited brilliantly by “reality TV” programmers, who cast television shows with real people, not actors, and place them in unusual or even difficult situations. This new genre of television show has proved a powerhouse.
Since the original version of Survivor (2000), reality shows have crowded the top ten list of most watched shows every year.
Why are these shows so popular with the American public? Because we enjoy figuring people out.
Social Perception

The study of how we form impressions of and make inferences about other people.

Social Perception

*

Nonverbal Behavior

Nonverbal Communication

The way in which people communicate, intentionally or unintentionally, without words.

Nonverbal cues include:

facial expressions
tone of voice
gestures
body position/movement
the use of touch
gaze
*

Nonverbal Behavior

We have a special kind of brain cell called mirror neurons.
These neurons respond when we perform an action and when we see someone else perform the same action.
Mirror neurons appear to be the basis of our ability to feel empathy.
For example, when we see someone crying, these mirror neurons fire automatically and involuntarily, just as if we were crying ourselves.
*

(Gallese, Fadiga, Fogassi & Rizzolatti, 1996)
Nonverbal Behavior

Nonverbal cues serve many functions in communication.

You can express “I’m angry” by narrowing your eyes, lowering your eyebrows, and setting your mouth in a thin, straight line.
You can convey the attitude “I like you” with smiles and extended eye contact.
And you communicate your personality traits, like being an extrovert, with broad gestures and frequent changes in voice pitch and inflection.
*

(Knapp & Hall, 2006)
Nonverbal Behavior

Some nonverbal cues actually contradict the spoken words.

Communicating sarcasm is the classic example of verbal-nonverbal contradiction.
Think about how you’d say “I’m so happy for you” sarcastically.
*

(DePaulo, 1992; Knapp & Hall, 2006))
Nonverbal cues can also substitute for the verbal message. Hand gestures such as flashing the “OK” sign or drawing a finger across your throat convey clear messages without any words at all (Ekman, 1965).
Facial Expressions of Emotion

Are facial expressions of emotion universal?

The answer is yes, for the six major emotional expressions: anger, happiness, surprise, fear, disgust, and sadness.

All humans encode or express these “six basic emotions” in the same way, and all humans can decode or interpret them with equal accuracy.

*

Darwin (1872) argued that such facial expressions then acquired evolutionary significance; being able to communicate such emotional states (e.g., the feeling of disgust, not for food but for another person or a situation) had survival value for the developing species (Hansen & Hansen, 1988; Izard, 1994; McArthur & Baron, 1983).
Facial Expressions of Emotion

Paul Ekman (www.paulekman.com) and others have conducted numerous studies indicating that the ability to interpret at least the six basic emotions is cross-cultural—part of being human and not a product of people’s cultural experience.
How did Ekman do this? He and Walter Friesen traveled to New Guinea in 1971, where they studied the nonverbal decoding ability of the South Fore, a preliterate tribe that had no contact with Western civilization. South Fore tribe members could accurately decode the six basic emotions when they were expressed by Westerners in photographs. (Westerners could do the same with photos of South Fore members.)
*

Ekman and Friesen (1971) traveled to New Guinea, where they studied the decoding ability of the South Fore, a preliterate tribe that had had no contact with Western civilization. They told the Fore people brief stories with emotional content and then showed them photographs of American men and women expressing the six emotions; the Fore’s job was to match the facial expressions of emotion to the stories. They were as accurate as Western subjects had been.
(Biehl et al., 1997; Ekman, 1993, 1994; Ekman et al., 1987; Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002; Izard, 1994)
Facial Expressions of Emotion

These photographs depict facial expressions of the six basic universal emotions (happiness, surprise, fear, sadness, anger, and disgust).
*

Ekman and Friesen (1971) traveled to New Guinea, where they studied the decoding ability of the South Fore, a preliterate tribe that had had no contact with Western civilization. They told the Fore people brief stories with emotional content and then showed them photographs of American men and women expressing the six emotions; the Fore’s job was to match the facial expressions of emotion to the stories. They were as accurate as Western subjects had been.
(Biehl et al., 1997; Ekman, 1993, 1994; Ekman et al., 1987; Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002; Izard, 1994)
Facial Expressions of Emotion

Other emotions such as guilt, shame, embarrassment, and pride occur later in human development and show less universality.
These latter emotions are closely tied to social interaction.
Some researchers contend that contempt is a 7th universally recognized emotion.
*

The six major emotions are also the first to appear in human development. Children as young as six months to a year express these emotions with the facial expressions we associate with adults. This is true as well for young children who have been blind from birth; they are able to encode the basic emotions even though they have never seen them displayed by adults (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1975; Galati, Miceli & Sini, 2001).
Facial Expressions of Emotion

Decoding facial expressions accurately is more complicated than we have indicated, for three reasons.

Affect blends occur when one part of the face registers one emotion and another part of the face registers a different emotion.

At times people try to appear less emotional than they are, so that no one will know how they really feel.

A third reason why decoding facial expressions can be inaccurate has to do with culture.

*

By suppressing your emotional response, you are not giving your tormentor the satisfaction of knowing he or she has upset you. Psychologists have studied what happens when people suppress their negative facial expressions; their results present an interesting cautionary tale (Gross 1998; Gross & Levenson, 1993, 1997).
Culture and the Channels
of Nonverbal Communication

Display rules are particular to each culture and dictate what kinds of emotional expressions people are supposed to show.

*

For decades, Paul Ekman and his colleagues have studied the influence of culture on the facial display of emotions (Ekman & Davidson, 1994; Ekman & Friesen, 1969; Matsumoto & Ekman, 1989; Matsumoto & Kudoh, 1993). They have concluded that display rules are particular to each culture and dictate what kinds of emotional expressions people are supposed to show.
Examples of display rule differences:

American cultural norms discourage emotional displays in men, such as grief or crying, but allow the facial display of such emotions in women.
Japanese women will often hide a wide smile behind their hands, whereas Western women are allowed—indeed, encouraged—to smile broadly and often.
Japanese norms lead people to cover up negative facial expressions with smiles and laughter and to display fewer facial expressions in general than is true in the West.
Culture and the Channels
of Nonverbal Communication

*

(Henley, 1977; La France, Hecht, & Paluck, 2003)
(Argyle, 1986; Aune & Aune, 1996; Gudykunst, Ting-Toomey, & Nishida, 1996; Richmond & McCroskey, 1995)
Members of American culture become suspicious when a person doesn’t “look them in the eye” while speaking, and they find talking to someone who is wearing dark sunglasses quite disconcerting.
Cultures vary greatly in what is considered normative use of personal space. Most Americans like to have a bubble of open space, a few feet in radius, surrounding them; in comparison, in some other cultures, strangers think nothing of standing right next to each other, to the point of touching.
Culture and the Channels
of Nonverbal Communication

*

(Hall, 1969)
The important point about emblems is that they are not universal.

Each culture has devised its own emblems, and these need not be understandable to people from other cultures.

Emblems

Nonverbal gestures that have well-understood definitions within a given culture; they usually have direct verbal translations, like the “OK” sign.

President George H. W. Bush once used the “V for victory” sign, but he did it backward—the palm of his hand was facing him instead of the audience. Unfortunately, he flashed this gesture to a large crowd in Australia—and in Australia, this emblem is the equivalent of “flipping the bird”!

Culture and the Channels
of Nonverbal Communication

*

(Archer, 1997a)
Multichannel Nonverbal Communication

Because nonverbal information is diffused across these many channels, we often rely on multiple channels to understand what is going on.
This increases our ability to make accurate judgments about others.
*

Except for certain specific situations, such as talking on the telephone, everyday life is made up of multichannel nonverbal social interaction.
Typically, many nonverbal cues are available to us when we talk to or observe other people.
How do we use this information?
And how accurately do we use it?
Multichannel Nonverbal Communication

*

(Archer & Akert, 1998; Rosenthal, Hall, Di Matteo, Rogers, & Archer, 1979)
Gender and Nonverbal Communication

In general, women are much better at encoding (or “sending”) and decoding (or “judging”) nonverbal cues (Hall, 1979, 1984).
One exception is that women are not more accurate than men at detecting deception. Rosenthal and DePaulo (1979) found that women lost their advantage at decoding nonverbal cues for those cues that the sender did not mean to encode. They concluded that women are more “nonverbally accommodating” than men (on average).
*

Many studies have found that women are better at both decoding and encoding (Hall, 1979, 1984; Rosenthal & De Paulo, 1979).
Social role theory of sex differences suggests that this is because women have learned different skills, and one is to be polite and overlook lying.
Given that women are generally superior decoders, why do they lose their advantage when faced with deceit? It may be because women are more polite than men. While women have the ability to decode nonverbal cues of lying, they tend to turn off this skill in the face of deception, in polite deference to the speaker (Rosenthal & De Paulo, 1979).
Gender and Nonverbal Communication

*

According to Alice Eagly’s social role theory, most societies have a division of labor based on gender:

Men work in jobs outside the home.
Women work within the home.
Gender and Nonverbal Communication

*

Alice Eagly (1987)
This division of labor has important consequences.

First, gender-role expectations arise:
Members of the society expect men and women to have certain attributes that are consistent with their role. Thus women are expected to be more nurturing, friendly, expressive, and sensitive than men because of their primary role as caregivers to children and elderly family members.

Gender and Nonverbal Communication

*

(Eagly & Karau, 2002).
This division of labor has important consequences.

Second, men and women develop different sets of skills and attitudes, based on their experiences in their gender roles.
Gender and Nonverbal Communication

*

(Barrett, Lane, Sechrest, & Schwartz, 2000)
This division of labor has important consequences.

Second, men and women develop different sets of skills and attitudes, based on their experiences in their gender roles.
Finally, because women are less powerful in many societies and less likely to occupy roles of higher status, it becomes more important for women to learn to be accommodating and polite than it is for men.
Gender and Nonverbal Communication

*

(Deaux & Major, 1987; Henley, 1977)
According to Eagly (1987), gender- role expectations and sex-typed skills combine to produce sex differences in social behavior, such as the differences in nonverbal behavior we just discussed.
This is exactly what Judith Hall (1979) found in her cross-cultural study of nonverbal behavior .
Implicit Personality Theories:
Filling in the Blanks

To understand other people, we observe their behavior but we also infer their feelings, traits, and motives.
To do so, we use general notions or schemas about which personality traits go together in one person.
*

A schema is a mental shortcut: When all we have is a small amount of information, our schemas provide additional information to fill in the gaps (Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Markus & Zajonc, 1985).
Thus when we are trying to understand other people, we can use just a few observations of a person as a starting point and then, using our schemas, create a much fuller understanding (Dweck, Chiu, & Hong, 1995; Kim & Rosenberg, 1980).
Schemas allow us to form impressions quickly, without having to spend weeks with people to figure out what they are like.
Implicit Personality Theory

A type of schema people use to group various kinds of personality traits together; for example, many people believe that someone who is kind is generous as well.

If someone is kind, our implicit personality theory tells us he or she is probably generous as well.
Similarly, we assume that a stingy person is also irritable.
Implicit Personality Theories:
Filling in the Blanks

*

(Asch, 1946; Schneider, 1973; Sedikides & Anderson, 1994; Werth & Foerster, 2002)
But relying on schemas can also lead us astray.

We might make the wrong assumptions about an individual.
We might even resort to stereotypical thinking, where our schema, or stereotype, leads us to believe that the individual is like all the other members of his or her group.
Implicit Personality Theories:
Filling in the Blanks

*

Culture and Implicit
Personality Theories

These general notions, or schemas, are shared by people in a culture, and are passed from one generation to another.

*

Like other beliefs, implicit personality theories are passed from generation to generation in a society, and one culture’s implicit personality theory may be very different from another’s (Anderson, 1995; Chiu, Morris, Hong, & Menon, 2000; Cousins, 1989; Vonk, 1995).
Culture and Implicit
Personality Theories

A strong implicit personality theory in this culture involves physical attractiveness. We presume that “what is beautiful is good”—that people with physical beauty will also have a whole host of other wonderful qualities.

*

(Dion, Berscheid, & Walster, 1972; Eagly, Ashmore, Makhijani, & Longo, 1991; Jackson, Hunter, & Hodge, 1995)
Culture and Implicit
Personality Theories

In China, one example of an implicit personality theory describes a person who embodies traditional Chinese values: creating and maintaining interpersonal harmony, inner harmony, and ren qin (a focus on relationships).

*

(Cheung et al., 1996)
Culture and Implicit
Personality Theories

In Western cultures, saying someone has an “artistic personality” implies that the person is creative, intense, and temperamental and has an unconventional lifestyle.

The Chinese, however, do not have a schema or implicit personality theory for an “artistic type.”

*

Culture and Implicit
Personality Theories

Conversely, in China, there are categories of personality that do not exist in Western cultures.

For example, a “shi gú” person is someone who is worldly, devoted to his or her family, socially skillful, and somewhat reserved. People in Western cultures do not recognize these as traits that “go together.”

*

Causal Attribution:
Answering the “Why” Question

If an acquaintance says, “It’s great to see you!” does she really mean it?

The point is that even though nonverbal communication is sometimes easy to decode and our implicit personality theories can streamline the way we form impressions, there is still substantial ambiguity as to what a person’s behavior really means.

Perhaps she is acting more thrilled than she really feels, out of politeness.
Perhaps she is outright lying and really can’t stand you!
*

(De Paulo, 1992; De Paulo, Stone, & Lassiter, 1985; Schneider, Hastorf, & Ellsworth, 1979)
Causal Attribution:
Answering the “Why” Question

According to attribution theory, we try to determine why people do what they do in order to uncover the feelings and traits that are behind their actions.

This helps us understand and predict our social world.

*

Why did that acquaintance behave as she did? To answer this “why” question, we will use our immediate observations to form more elegant and complex inferences about what people are really like and what motivates them to act as they do.
How we go about answering these questions is the focus of attribution theory, the study of how we infer the causes of other people’s behavior.
The Nature of the Attribution Process

Fritz Heider (1958) is frequently referred to as the father of attribution theory.

Heider discussed what he called “naive” or “commonsense” psychology.

In his view, people were like amateur scientists, trying to understand other people’s behavior by piecing together information until they arrived at a reasonable explanation or cause.

Heider was intrigued by what seemed reasonable to people and by how they arrived at their conclusions.

*

His influential book defined the field of social perception, and his legacy is still very much evident in current research (Gilbert, 1998a; Ross, 1998).
The Nature of the Attribution Process

When trying to decide what causes people’s behavior, we can make one of two attributions:

An internal, dispositional attribution or
An external, situational attribution.
*

One of Heider’s most valuable contributions is a simple dichotomy: When trying to decide why people behave as they do—for example, why a father has just yelled at his young daughter—we can make one of two attributions. One option is to make an internal attribution, deciding that the cause of the father’s behavior was something about him—his disposition, personality, attitudes, or character—an explanation that assigns the causes of his behavior internally. For example, we might decide that the father has poor parenting skills and disciplines his child in inappropriate ways. Alternatively, we might make an external attribution, deciding that something in the situation, not in the father’s personality or attitudes, caused his behavior. If we conclude that he yelled because his daughter had just stepped into the street without looking, we would be making an external attribution for his behavior.
Internal Attribution:

The inference that a person is behaving in a certain way because of something about the person, such as his/her attitude, character, or personality.

External Attribution:

The inference that a person is behaving a certain way because of something about the situation he or she is in.

The assumption is that most people would respond the same way in that situation.

*

The Nature of the Attribution Process

Satisfied spouses tend to show one pattern:

Internal attributions for their partners’ positive behaviors (e.g., “She helped me because she’s such a generous person”).
External attributions for their partners’ negative behaviors (e.g., “He said something mean because he’s so stressed at work this week”).
In contrast, spouses in distressed marriages tend to display the opposite pattern:

Their partners’ positive behaviors are chalked up to external causes (e.g., “She helped me because she wanted to impress our friends”).
Negative behaviors are attributed to internal causes (e.g., “He said something mean because he’s a totally self-centered jerk”).
*

When an intimate relationship becomes troubled, this second pattern of attributions about one’s partner only makes the situation worse and can have dire consequences for the health and future of the relationship (Bradbury & Fincham, 1991; Fincham, Bradbury, Arias, Byrne, & Karney, 1997; Karney & Bradbury, 2000).
The Nature of the Attribution Process

Although either type of attribution is always possible, Heider (1958) noted that we tend to see the causes of a person’s behavior as residing in that person (internal explanation).

We are perceptually focused on people—they are who we notice.
The situation (the external explanation), which is often hard to see and hard to describe, may be overlooked.
*

(Bargh, 1994; Fletcher, Reeder, & Bull, 1990; Gilbert, 1998b; Jones, 1979, 1990; Jones & Davis, 1965; Miller, 1998)
The Covariation Model:
Internal versus External Attributions

Harold Kelley’s major contribution to attribution theory was the idea that we notice and think about more than one piece of information when we form an impression of another person.

Covariation Model

A theory that states that to form an attribution about what caused a person’s behavior, we systematically note the pattern between the presence or absence of possible causal factors and whether or not the behavior occurs.

*

Kelley (1967, 1973)
The Covariation Model:
Internal versus External Attributions

The covariation model focuses on observations of behavior across time, place, actors, and targets.

It examines how the perceiver chooses either an internal or an external attribution.

We make such choices by using information on:

Consensus,
Distinctiveness,
Consistency.
*

Kelley, like Heider before him, assumes that when we are in the process of forming an attribution, we gather information, or data.
The data we use, according to Kelley, are how a person’s behavior “covaries” or changes across time, place, different actors, and different targets of the behavior.
By discovering covariation in people’s behavior (e.g., your friend refuses to lend you her car; she agrees to lend it to others), you are able to reach a judgment about what caused their behavior.
Consensus Information

Information about the extent to which other people behave the same way toward the same stimulus as the actor does.

Distinctiveness Information

Information about the extent to which one particular actor behaves in the same way to different stimuli.

Consistency Information

Information about the extent to which the behavior between one actor and one stimulus is the same across time and circumstances.

*

The Correspondence Bias:
People as Personality Psychologists

Specific errors or biases plague the attribution process.

One common shortcut is the correspondence bias:

the tendency to believe that people’s behavior matches (corresponds to) their dispositions.

*

(Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Gilbert, 1998b; Gilbert & Jones, 1986; Gilbert & Malone, 1995; Jones, 1979, 1990)
The Correspondence Bias:
People as Personality Psychologists

The pervasive, fundamental theory or schema most of us have about human behavior is that people do what they do because of the kind of people they are, not because of the situation they are in.
When thinking this way, we are more like personality psychologists, who see behavior as stemming from internal dispositions and traits, than like social psychologists, who focus on the impact of social situations on behavior.
*

The Correspondence Bias:
People as Personality Psychologists

The correspondence bias is so pervasive that many social psychologists call it the fundamental attribution error.

*

(Heider, 1958; Jones, 1990; Ross, 1977; Ross & Nisbett, 1991)
The Correspondence Bias:
People as Personality Psychologists

One reason is that when we try to explain someone’s behavior, our focus of attention is usually on the person, not on the surrounding situation.

If we don’t know someone got an “F” on a test earlier in the day, we can’t use that situational information to help us understand her current behavior.

And even when we know her situation, we still don’t know how she interprets it.

The “F” may not have upset her if she’s planning to drop the course anyway.

*

(Baron & Misovich, 1993; Heider, 1944, 1958; Jones & Nisbett, 1972)
(Gilbert, 1998b; Gilbert & Malone, 1995)
The Correspondence Bias:
People as Personality Psychologists

We can’t see the situation, so we ignore its importance.

People, not the situation, have perceptual salience for us.

We pay attention to them, and we tend to think that they alone cause their behavior.

Perceptual Salience

The seeming importance of information that is the focus of people’s attention.

*

The Correspondence Bias:
People as Personality Psychologists

The culprit is one of the mental shortcuts we discussed in chapter 3: the anchoring and adjustment heuristic.

The correspondence bias is another byproduct of this shortcut.

When making attributions, people use the focus of their attention as a starting point.

*

The Two-Step Process

In sum, we go through a two-step process when we make attributions.

First, we make an internal attribution; we assume that a person’s behavior was due to something about that person.

Then we attempt to adjust this attribution by considering the situation the person was in. But we often don’t make enough of an adjustment in this second step.

*

(Gilbert, 1989, 1991, 1993; Krull, 1993)
Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education. All rights reserved.

*

The Two-Step Process

In sum, we go through a two-step process when we make attributions.

First, we make an internal attribution; we assume that a person’s behavior was due to something about that person.

Then we attempt to adjust this attribution by considering the situation the person was in. But we often don’t make enough of an adjustment in this second step.

Why? Because the first step occurs quickly and spontaneously whereas the second step requires more effort and conscious attention.

*

(Gilbert, 1989, 1991, 1993; Krull, 1993)
The Two-Step Process

We will engage in the second step of attributional processing if we:

Consciously slow down and think carefully before reaching a judgment,
Are motivated to reach as accurate a judgment as possible, or
Are suspicious about the behavior of the target person (e.g., we suspect lying).
*

(Burger, 1991; Fein, 1996; Hilton, Fein, & Miller, 1993; Webster, 1993)
Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education. All rights reserved.

Adapted from Taylor & Fiske, 1975.
*

This is the seating arrangement for two actors and the six research participants in the Taylor & Fiske study. People rated the actor they could see more clearly as having the larger role in the conversation.

Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education. All rights reserved.

Adapted from Taylor & Fiske, 1975.
*

These are the ratings of each actor’s perceived causal role in the conversation (adapted from Taylor & Fiske, 1975).

Culture and the Correspondence Bias

For decades, it was taken for granted that the correspondence bias was universal:

People everywhere, we thought, applied this cognitive shortcut when forming attributions.

*

(Norenzayan, Choi, & Nisbett, 1999)
People from individualistic and collectivistic cultures do both demonstrate the correspondence bias.

However, members of collectivist cultures are more sensitive to situational causes of behavior and more likely to rely on situational explanations, as long as situational variables are salient.

Culture and the Correspondence Bias

*

North American and some other Western cultures stress individual autonomy. A person is perceived as independent and self-contained; his or her behavior reflects internal traits, motives, and values.

In contrast, East Asian cultures such as those in China, Japan, and Korea stress group autonomy. The individual derives his or her sense of self from the social group to which he or she belongs.

Culture and the Correspondence Bias

*

(Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Menon et al., 1999, p. 703)
It would be a mistake to think that members of collectivist cultures don’t make dispositional attributions.

They do—it’s just a matter of degree. They do it a little less than people in individualist cultures.

Culture and the Correspondence Bias

*

Recent research indicates that a tendency to think dispositionally about others—the correspondence bias—appears in many cultures.
However, members of collectivistic cultures are more aware of how the situation affects behavior and more likely to take situational effects into account (Choi, Dalal, Kim-Prieto, & Park, 2003; Choi & Nisbett, 1998; Choi, Nisbett, & Norenzayan, 1999; Krull et al., 1999; Miyamoto & Kitayama, 2002).
The Actor/Observer Difference

The actor-observer difference is an amplification of the correspondence bias:

We tend to see other people’s behavior as dispositionally caused, while we are more likely to see our own behavior as situationally caused.

The effect occurs because perceptual salience and information availability differ for the actor and the observer.

*

(Jones & Nisbett, 1972; Hansen, Kimble, & Biers, 2001; Nisbett, Caputo, Legant, & Marecek, 1973; Robins, Spranka, & Mendelson, 1996; Watson, 1982)
The Actor/Observer Difference

Actors have more information about themselves than observers do.

Actors know how they’ve behaved over the years; they know what happened to them that morning.

They are far more aware than observers are of both the similarities and the differences in their behavior over time and across situations terms, actors have far more consistency and distinctiveness information about themselves than observers do.

*

(Greenwald & Banaji, 1989; Jones & Nisbett, 1972; Krueger, Ham, & Linford, 1996; Malle & Knobe, 1997)
The Actor/Observer Difference

For example: Let’s say that you saw someone slip on a banana peel. What attribution might you make? Probably that he was clumsy. But let’s say that you slipped on a banana peel. Wouldn’t you be more likely to make an attribution about the situation in that case?

*

(Greenwald & Banaji, 1989; Jones & Nisbett, 1972; Krueger, Ham, & Linford, 1996; Malle & Knobe, 1997)
Self-Serving Attributions

Self-Serving Attributions

Explanations for one’s successes that credit internal, dispositional factors and explanations for one’s failures that blame external, situational factors.

Defensive Attributions

Explanations for behavior that avoid feelings of vulnerability and mortality.

Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education. All rights reserved.

*

Self-Serving Attributions

Why do we make self-serving attributions?

Most people try to maintain their self-esteem whenever possible, even if that means distorting reality by changing a thought or belief.
We are particularly likely to engage in self-serving attributions when we fail at something and we feel we can’t improve at it.
The external attribution truly protects our self-esteem, as there is little hope we can do better in the future.
But if we believe we can improve, we’re more likely to attribute our current failure to internal causes and then work on improving.
*

(Duval & Silvia, 2002)
Self-Serving Attributions

Why do we make self-serving attributions?

Most people try to maintain their self-esteem whenever possible, even if that means distorting reality by changing a thought or belief.
We want people to think well of us and to admire us. Telling others that our poor performance was due to some external cause puts a “good face” on failure; many people call this strategy “making excuses.”
*

(Greenberg et al., 1982; Tetlock, 1981; Weary & Arkin, 1981)
Self-Serving Attributions

Why do we make self-serving attributions?

Most people try to maintain their self-esteem whenever possible, even if that means distorting reality by changing a thought or belief.

We want people to think well of us and to admire us. Telling others that our poor performance was due to some external cause puts a “good face” on failure; many people call this strategy “making excuses.”

We know more about our own efforts than we do about other people’s.

*

Let’s imagine the attributional process of another student in the chemistry class, Ron, who did poorly on the midterm. Ron knows that he studied very hard for the midterm, that he typically does well on chemistry tests, and that in general he is a very good student. The D on the chemistry midterm comes as a surprise. The most logical attribution Ron can make is that the test was unfair—the D grade wasn’t due to a lack of ability or effort. The professor, however, knows that some students did well on the test; given the information that is available to the professor, it is logical for him to conclude that Ron, and not the fact that it was a difficult test, was responsible for the poor grade (Miller & Ross, 1975; Nisbett & Ross, 1980).
Self-Serving Attributions

One form of defensive attribution is to believe that bad things happen only to bad people or at least, only to people who make stupid mistakes or poor choices.
Therefore, bad things won’t happen to us because we won’t be that stupid or careless.
Melvin Lerner called this the belief in a just world—the assumption that people get what they deserve and deserve what they get.
*

Lerner (1980; 1998)
(Hafer, 2000; Hafer & Begue, 2005; Lipkus, Dalbert, & Siegler, 1996)
Self-Serving Attributions

The just world belief has unfortunate consequences:

Victims of crimes or accidents are often seen as causing their own fate.
People tend to believe that rape victims are to blame for the rape.
Battered wives are often seen as responsible for their abusive husbands’ actions.
*

Not only do people tend to believe that rape victims are to blame for the rape (Abrams, Viki, Masser, & Bohner, 2003; Bell, Kuriloff, & Lottes, 1994), but battered wives are often seen as responsible for their abusive husbands’ behavior (Summers & Feldman, 1984).
Self-Serving Attributions

Example of a self-serving attribution:

Please rate your sense of humor on a scale from “1” (very poor sense of humor) to “9” (excellent sense of humor) now. Then read below.

------

In any large group of people, you will find most people rate themselves above the midpoint (which would be “5”). In other words, the average person thinks he/she is better than average; this is true with sense of humor, or intelligence, or almost any other positive trait. This is also known as the “better-than-average” effect.

*

Culture and Other
Attributional Biases

There is some evidence for cross-cultural differences in the Actor-Observer Effect and in Self-Serving and Defensive Attributions.

Typically, the difference occurs between Western, individualistic cultures and Eastern, collectivistic cultures.

*

How Accurate Are Our Attributions and Impressions?

Our impressions are sometimes wrong because of the mental shortcuts we use when forming social judgments.

To improve the accuracy of your attributions, remember that the mental shortcuts we use, such as the correspondence bias, can lead us to the wrong conclusions sometimes.

Even with such biases operating, we are quite accurate perceivers of other people.

We do very well most of the time.

In fact, most of us are more accurate than we realize.

*

(Funder, 1995; Kenny, Albright, Malloy, & Kashy, 1994).
(Ambady, Bernieri, & Richeson, 2000; Archer & Akert, 1980; Costanzo & Archer, 1989)
In conclusion:

We are capable of making both stunningly accurate assessments of people and horrific attributional mistakes.

Chapter 4 Websites to Explore

Websites of famous nonverbal communication researchers:
http://www.paulekman.com/
http://www.belladepaulo.com/deception.htm
http://nonverbal.ucsc.edu/
http://www.port.ac.uk/departments/academic/psychology/staff/title,50475,en.html
http://www.rap.ucr.edu/
http://campus.usal.es/~nonverbal/researchers.htm
http://euphrates.wpunj.edu/faculty/wagnerk/webagogy/hecht.htm
Website for nonverbal communication research taking place here at NSU:
http://www.nova.edu/~sterngla/research.html
Microexpressions tutorial from the FOX-TV show “Lie to Me” (based very loosely on Ekman’s research -- but not everything in the show is scientifically accurate):
http://www.hulu.com/watch/53632/lie-to-me-expressions---introduction
Interesting nonverbal communication journal articles:
http://www.belladepaulo.com/deceptionpubs.htm
http://www.paulekman.com/publications/journal-articles-book-chapters/
http://www.port.ac.uk/departments/academic/psychology/staff/downloads/filetodownload,113333,en.pdf
http://www.springerlink.com/content/v7l182w3083688w8/
Interesting demonstration of the fundamental attribution error:
http://courses.ttu.edu/hdfs3390-reifman/deandemo.htm
Chapter 4 Discussion

To what extent is nonverbal communication universal, and to what extent is nonverbal communication culturally based?
Please be specific. You can choose to discuss any aspect of nonverbal communication (including communication of various emotions, deception, etc.)
*

*

*

This basic aspect of human cognition has been exploited brilliantly by “reality TV” programmers, who cast television shows with real people, not actors, and place them in unusual or even difficult situations. This new genre of television show has proved a powerhouse.
Since the original version of Survivor (2000), reality shows have crowded the top ten list of most watched shows every year.
Why are these shows so popular with the American public? Because we enjoy figuring people out.
*

*

*

(Gallese, Fadiga, Fogassi & Rizzolatti, 1996)
*

(Knapp & Hall, 2006)
*

(DePaulo, 1992; Knapp & Hall, 2006))
Nonverbal cues can also substitute for the verbal message. Hand gestures such as flashing the “OK” sign or drawing a finger across your throat convey clear messages without any words at all (Ekman, 1965).
*

Darwin (1872) argued that such facial expressions then acquired evolutionary significance; being able to communicate such emotional states (e.g., the feeling of disgust, not for food but for another person or a situation) had survival value for the developing species (Hansen & Hansen, 1988; Izard, 1994; McArthur & Baron, 1983).
*

Ekman and Friesen (1971) traveled to New Guinea, where they studied the decoding ability of the South Fore, a preliterate tribe that had had no contact with Western civilization. They told the Fore people brief stories with emotional content and then showed them photographs of American men and women expressing the six emotions; the Fore’s job was to match the facial expressions of emotion to the stories. They were as accurate as Western subjects had been.
(Biehl et al., 1997; Ekman, 1993, 1994; Ekman et al., 1987; Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002; Izard, 1994)
*

Ekman and Friesen (1971) traveled to New Guinea, where they studied the decoding ability of the South Fore, a preliterate tribe that had had no contact with Western civilization. They told the Fore people brief stories with emotional content and then showed them photographs of American men and women expressing the six emotions; the Fore’s job was to match the facial expressions of emotion to the stories. They were as accurate as Western subjects had been.
(Biehl et al., 1997; Ekman, 1993, 1994; Ekman et al., 1987; Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002; Izard, 1994)
*

The six major emotions are also the first to appear in human development. Children as young as six months to a year express these emotions with the facial expressions we associate with adults. This is true as well for young children who have been blind from birth; they are able to encode the basic emotions even though they have never seen them displayed by adults (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1975; Galati, Miceli & Sini, 2001).
*

By suppressing your emotional response, you are not giving your tormentor the satisfaction of knowing he or she has upset you. Psychologists have studied what happens when people suppress their negative facial expressions; their results present an interesting cautionary tale (Gross 1998; Gross & Levenson, 1993, 1997).
*

For decades, Paul Ekman and his colleagues have studied the influence of culture on the facial display of emotions (Ekman & Davidson, 1994; Ekman & Friesen, 1969; Matsumoto & Ekman, 1989; Matsumoto & Kudoh, 1993). They have concluded that display rules are particular to each culture and dictate what kinds of emotional expressions people are supposed to show.
*

(Henley, 1977; La France, Hecht, & Paluck, 2003)
(Argyle, 1986; Aune & Aune, 1996; Gudykunst, Ting-Toomey, & Nishida, 1996; Richmond & McCroskey, 1995)
*

(Hall, 1969)
*

(Archer, 1997a)
*

*

(Archer & Akert, 1998; Rosenthal, Hall, Di Matteo, Rogers, & Archer, 1979)
*

Many studies have found that women are better at both decoding and encoding (Hall, 1979, 1984; Rosenthal & De Paulo, 1979).
*

*

Alice Eagly (1987)
*

(Eagly & Karau, 2002).
*

(Barrett, Lane, Sechrest, & Schwartz, 2000)
*

(Deaux & Major, 1987; Henley, 1977)
According to Eagly (1987), gender- role expectations and sex-typed skills combine to produce sex differences in social behavior, such as the differences in nonverbal behavior we just discussed.
This is exactly what Judith Hall (1979) found in her cross-cultural study of nonverbal behavior .
*

A schema is a mental shortcut: When all we have is a small amount of information, our schemas provide additional information to fill in the gaps (Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Markus & Zajonc, 1985).
Thus when we are trying to understand other people, we can use just a few observations of a person as a starting point and then, using our schemas, create a much fuller understanding (Dweck, Chiu, & Hong, 1995; Kim & Rosenberg, 1980).
Schemas allow us to form impressions quickly, without having to spend weeks with people to figure out what they are like.
*

(Asch, 1946; Schneider, 1973; Sedikides & Anderson, 1994; Werth & Foerster, 2002)
*

*

Like other beliefs, implicit personality theories are passed from generation to generation in a society, and one culture’s implicit personality theory may be very different from another’s (Anderson, 1995; Chiu, Morris, Hong, & Menon, 2000; Cousins, 1989; Vonk, 1995).
*

(Dion, Berscheid, & Walster, 1972; Eagly, Ashmore, Makhijani, & Longo, 1991; Jackson, Hunter, & Hodge, 1995)
*

(Cheung et al., 1996)
*

*

*

(De Paulo, 1992; De Paulo, Stone, & Lassiter, 1985; Schneider, Hastorf, & Ellsworth, 1979)
*

Why did that acquaintance behave as she did? To answer this “why” question, we will use our immediate observations to form more elegant and complex inferences about what people are really like and what motivates them to act as they do.
How we go about answering these questions is the focus of attribution theory, the study of how we infer the causes of other people’s behavior.
*

His influential book defined the field of social perception, and his legacy is still very much evident in current research (Gilbert, 1998a; Ross, 1998).
*

One of Heider’s most valuable contributions is a simple dichotomy: When trying to decide why people behave as they do—for example, why a father has just yelled at his young daughter—we can make one of two attributions. One option is to make an internal attribution, deciding that the cause of the father’s behavior was something about him—his disposition, personality, attitudes, or character—an explanation that assigns the causes of his behavior internally. For example, we might decide that the father has poor parenting skills and disciplines his child in inappropriate ways. Alternatively, we might make an external attribution, deciding that something in the situation, not in the father’s personality or attitudes, caused his behavior. If we conclude that he yelled because his daughter had just stepped into the street without looking, we would be making an external attribution for his behavior.
*

*

When an intimate relationship becomes troubled, this second pattern of attributions about one’s partner only makes the situation worse and can have dire consequences for the health and future of the relationship (Bradbury & Fincham, 1991; Fincham, Bradbury, Arias, Byrne, & Karney, 1997; Karney & Bradbury, 2000).
*

(Bargh, 1994; Fletcher, Reeder, & Bull, 1990; Gilbert, 1998b; Jones, 1979, 1990; Jones & Davis, 1965; Miller, 1998)
*

Kelley (1967, 1973)
*

Kelley, like Heider before him, assumes that when we are in the process of forming an attribution, we gather information, or data.
The data we use, according to Kelley, are how a person’s behavior “covaries” or changes across time, place, different actors, and different targets of the behavior.
By discovering covariation in people’s behavior (e.g., your friend refuses to lend you her car; she agrees to lend it to others), you are able to reach a judgment about what caused their behavior.
*

*

(Fiske & Taylor, 1991; Gilbert, 1998b; Gilbert & Jones, 1986; Gilbert & Malone, 1995; Jones, 1979, 1990)
*

*

(Heider, 1958; Jones, 1990; Ross, 1977; Ross & Nisbett, 1991)
*

(Baron & Misovich, 1993; Heider, 1944, 1958; Jones & Nisbett, 1972)
(Gilbert, 1998b; Gilbert & Malone, 1995)
*

*

*

(Gilbert, 1989, 1991, 1993; Krull, 1993)
*

*

(Gilbert, 1989, 1991, 1993; Krull, 1993)
*

(Burger, 1991; Fein, 1996; Hilton, Fein, & Miller, 1993; Webster, 1993)
*

This is the seating arrangement for two actors and the six research participants in the Taylor & Fiske study. People rated the actor they could see more clearly as having the larger role in the conversation.

*

These are the ratings of each actor’s perceived causal role in the conversation (adapted from Taylor & Fiske, 1975).

*

(Norenzayan, Choi, & Nisbett, 1999)
*

*

(Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Menon et al., 1999, p. 703)
*

Recent research indicates that a tendency to think dispositionally about others—the correspondence bias—appears in many cultures.
However, members of collectivistic cultures are more aware of how the situation affects behavior and more likely to take situational effects into account (Choi, Dalal, Kim-Prieto, & Park, 2003; Choi & Nisbett, 1998; Choi, Nisbett, & Norenzayan, 1999; Krull et al., 1999; Miyamoto & Kitayama, 2002).
*

(Jones & Nisbett, 1972; Hansen, Kimble, & Biers, 2001; Nisbett, Caputo, Legant, & Marecek, 1973; Robins, Spranka, & Mendelson, 1996; Watson, 1982)
*

(Greenwald & Banaji, 1989; Jones & Nisbett, 1972; Krueger, Ham, & Linford, 1996; Malle & Knobe, 1997)
*

(Greenwald & Banaji, 1989; Jones & Nisbett, 1972; Krueger, Ham, & Linford, 1996; Malle & Knobe, 1997)
*

*

(Duval & Silvia, 2002)
*

(Greenberg et al., 1982; Tetlock, 1981; Weary & Arkin, 1981)
*

Let’s imagine the attributional process of another student in the chemistry class, Ron, who did poorly on the midterm. Ron knows that he studied very hard for the midterm, that he typically does well on chemistry tests, and that in general he is a very good student. The D on the chemistry midterm comes as a surprise. The most logical attribution Ron can make is that the test was unfair—the D grade wasn’t due to a lack of ability or effort. The professor, however, knows that some students did well on the test; given the information that is available to the professor, it is logical for him to conclude that Ron, and not the fact that it was a difficult test, was responsible for the poor grade (Miller & Ross, 1975; Nisbett & Ross, 1980).
*

Lerner (1980; 1998)
(Hafer, 2000; Hafer & Begue, 2005; Lipkus, Dalbert, & Siegler, 1996)
*

Not only do people tend to believe that rape victims are to blame for the rape (Abrams, Viki, Masser, & Bohner, 2003; Bell, Kuriloff, & Lottes, 1994), but battered wives are often seen as responsible for their abusive husbands’ behavior (Summers & Feldman, 1984).
*

*

*

(Funder, 1995; Kenny, Albright, Malloy, & Kashy, 1994).
(Ambady, Bernieri, & Richeson, 2000; Archer & Akert, 1980; Costanzo & Archer, 1989)

Homework is Completed By:

Writer Writer Name Amount Client Comments & Rating
Instant Homework Helper

ONLINE

Instant Homework Helper

$36

She helped me in last minute in a very reasonable price. She is a lifesaver, I got A+ grade in my homework, I will surely hire her again for my next assignments, Thumbs Up!

Order & Get This Solution Within 3 Hours in $25/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 3 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

Order & Get This Solution Within 6 Hours in $20/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 6 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

Order & Get This Solution Within 12 Hours in $15/Page

Custom Original Solution And Get A+ Grades

  • 100% Plagiarism Free
  • Proper APA/MLA/Harvard Referencing
  • Delivery in 12 Hours After Placing Order
  • Free Turnitin Report
  • Unlimited Revisions
  • Privacy Guaranteed

6 writers have sent their proposals to do this homework:

Online Assignment Help
Top Class Results
George M.
Quick N Quality
Write My Coursework
Solution Provider
Writer Writer Name Offer Chat
Online Assignment Help

ONLINE

Online Assignment Help

I have written research reports, assignments, thesis, research proposals, and dissertations for different level students and on different subjects.

$42 Chat With Writer
Top Class Results

ONLINE

Top Class Results

As per my knowledge I can assist you in writing a perfect Planning, Marketing Research, Business Pitches, Business Proposals, Business Feasibility Reports and Content within your given deadline and budget.

$43 Chat With Writer
George M.

ONLINE

George M.

I have done dissertations, thesis, reports related to these topics, and I cover all the CHAPTERS accordingly and provide proper updates on the project.

$27 Chat With Writer
Quick N Quality

ONLINE

Quick N Quality

I have read your project description carefully and you will get plagiarism free writing according to your requirements. Thank You

$19 Chat With Writer
Write My Coursework

ONLINE

Write My Coursework

I am an academic and research writer with having an MBA degree in business and finance. I have written many business reports on several topics and am well aware of all academic referencing styles.

$18 Chat With Writer
Solution Provider

ONLINE

Solution Provider

I am a PhD writer with 10 years of experience. I will be delivering high-quality, plagiarism-free work to you in the minimum amount of time. Waiting for your message.

$18 Chat With Writer

Let our expert academic writers to help you in achieving a+ grades in your homework, assignment, quiz or exam.

Similar Homework Questions

Discussion Board - Triage assessment form - Screwtape letters chapter 31 - New novel complex decisions having no proven answers are - English Outline - Australian bank bill futures - Maslow's progression hypothesis states that: - Fugates of troublesome creek - Big blue bus 10 - Write in 500 words the disaster recovery plan and who is responsible at your place of employment. Consider the critical business functions and your recovery point objectives and recovery time objectives. - Nvr aqtf assessor requirements - Singapore airlines balancing act - Outreach networks first venture round - Separation of the components of a mixture lab report - Dulux 72yy 47 743 - Workplan - Acara work samples year 3 writing - HR Policy - New covenant jeremiah ezekiel - Multimedia fluid mechanics cambridge university press - Contemporary asian america a multidisciplinary reader - 841 wynnewood rd philadelphia pa 19151 - Magnets crossword puzzle answers - Essential cell biology 2010 - A share of stock with a beta of 75 - Organ Leader & Decision Making - Country manager latin america simulation answers - Falling down customer is always right - Olap cube tutorial pdf - James loder the transforming moment - Aca code of ethics apa citation - The key agencies regulating and enforcing rsa in nsw are: - Bellis fruit bars factory outlet - Esp 415 iii tns - Samantha graves facebook - The big trip up yonder answers - Journal week 4 - Scheme of work definition - Amp capital global property securities fund class a - Best interest duty checklist - 1 - Jitterbug perfume analysis - 8.COURSE NAME - MARKETING MANAGEMENT - Sesame seeds acidic or alkaline - Global poverty lesson plan - Nursing - Mcgraw hill connect economics homework answers - 15th century research - Sociology final exam questions and answers pdf - Det nsw net wifi - Texas assessment practice the poor and their betters answers - Ramón compró tres corbatas. roberto compró tres corbatas. - Law society formula b - Textual conversations richard iii and looking for richard - Pbis survey for teachers - Why does ice float in water - 555 california tree lighting - Develop a broad vision, an architecture, and a detailed plan of action that follows a life cycle concept for Developing IT Compliance Program - How did miss skiffins brother earn his living - NEED AN A+! Summative Assignment: Critique of Research Article NEED AN A+! - Esther park shadow health answers - Week 6 Final Paper and Folio - A sheet that is made of plastic n - The underground fat loss manual - Chapter 12 investing in stocks answers - Irac format case brief example - Lowest point of expected variation - Snoopy's dog breed crossword - Datamatics business solutions work from home - Pro and Con of an issue: Free Speech Regulated on social media - Advocacy role of nurse practitioner - Friends joey kisses monica - Discharge letter oet sample - To bid or not to bid - Basic types of energy - Rally base drop strategy - P-O-P - The social work skills workbook cournoyer pdf - Oregon earthquake hazard map - Spelling city com student login - Duty of care licensed premises - Data center grounding best practices - List of irregular verbs in english and spanish - Project Quality & HR Management - Yiyili community indigenous corporation - Block and tackle frame - Threats to a computerized accounting system - Withdraw without academic penalty uts - Komatsu axo 80 oil specifications - Healthcare Administration 2 pg Paper - Reflecting on Reading - Tesla motors swot analysis 2015 - Organizational behavior of google company - Ray white gen 5 - History of naa luro pdf - How the hr function varies between organisations in different sectors - Question about why social worker field - Graph for sin x cos y - Library management system slideshare - Aromatic ammonia uses