The Study of Language This best-selling textbook provides an engaging and user-friendly introduction to
the study of language. Assuming no prior knowledge of the subject, Yule presents
information in bite-sized sections, clearly explaining the major concepts in linguistics –
from how children learn language to why men and women speak differently, through
all the key elements of language. This fifth edition has been revised and updated
with new figures and tables, additional topics, and numerous new examples using
languages from across the world. To increase student engagement, and to foster
problem-solving and critical-thinking skills, the book includes thirty new tasks. An
expanded and revised online study guide provides students with further resources,
including answers and tutorials for all tasks, while encouraging lively and proactive
learning. This is the most fundamental and easy-to-use introduction to the study
of language.
George Yule has taught Linguistics at the universities of Edinburgh, Hawai‘i,
Louisiana State and Minnesota.
The Study of Language FIFTH EDITION
George Yule
University Printing House, Cambridge CB2 8BS, United Kingdom
Published in the United States of America by Cambridge University Press, New York
Cambridge University Press is part of the University of Cambridge.
It furthers the University’s mission by disseminating knowledge in the pursuit of
education, learning and research at the highest international levels of excellence.
www.cambridge.org
Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9781107658172
First and second editions © Cambridge University Press 1985, 1996 Third, fourth and fifth editions © George Yule 2006, 2010, 2014
This publication is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception
and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements,
no reproduction of any part may take place without the written
permission of Cambridge University Press.
First published 1985
Second edition 1996
Third edition 2006
Fourth edition 2010
Fifth edition 2014
Printed in the United Kingdom by MPG Printgroup Ltd, Cambridge
A catalogue record for this publication is available from the British Library
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Yule, George, 1947–
The study of language / George Yule. – 5th ed.
pages cm
Previous ed.: 2010.
ISBN 978-1-107-04419-7 (Hardback) – ISBN 978-1-107-65817-2 (Paperback)
1. Language and languages. 2. Linguistics. I. Title.
P107.Y85 2014
400–dc23 2013028557
ISBN 978-1-107-04419-7 Hardback
ISBN 978-1-107-65817-2 Paperback
Additional resources for this publication at www.cambridge.org/XXXXXXXXX
Cambridge University Press has no responsibility for the persistence or accuracy of
URLs for external or third-party internet websites referred to in this publication,
and does not guarantee that any content on such websites is, or will remain,
accurate or appropriate.
http://www.cambridge.org/XXXXXXXXX
http://www.cambridge.org/9781107658172
http://www.cambridge.org
Contents
Preface xi
l1 The origins of language The divine source 2 The natural sound source 2
The “bow-wow” theory 3 The “pooh-pooh” theory 3
The social interaction source 3 The physical adaptation source 4
Teeth and lips 4 Mouth and tongue 5 Larynx and pharynx 5
The tool-making source 5 The human brain 6
The genetic source 6 Study questions 8 Tasks 8 Discussion topics/projects 9 Further reading 9
l2 Animals and human language Communication 12 Properties of human language 12
Displacement 13 Arbitrariness 13 Productivity 14 Cultural transmission 15 Duality 16
Talking to animals 16 Chimpanzees and language 17
Washoe 17 Sarah 18 Lana 19 The controversy 19 Kanzi 20
Using language 20 Study questions 22 Tasks 22 Discussion topics/projects 23 Further reading 24
l3 The sounds of language Phonetics 27 Voiced and voiceless sounds 27 Place of articulation 27 Consonants 28
Familiar symbols 28 Unfamiliar symbols 29
Consonants: manner of articulation 29
Stops 29 Fricatives 30 Affricates 30 Nasals 31 Liquids 31 Glides 31
A consonant chart 31 Glottal stops and flaps 31
Vowels 32 Diphthongs 33 Subtle individual variation 34 Study questions 35 Tasks 35 Discussion topics/projects 37 Further reading 37
l4 The sound patterns of language Phonology 40 Phonemes 40
Natural classes 41 Phones and allophones 41 Minimal pairs and sets 42 Phonotactics 43 Syllables 43
Consonant clusters 44 Coarticulation effects 44
Assimilation 45 Nasalization 45 Elision 46 Normal speech 46
Study questions 47 Tasks 47 Discussion topics/projects 48 Bob Belviso translated 49 Further reading 49
l5 Word formation Neologisms 51 Etymology 51 Borrowing 52
Loan-translation 52 Compounding 53
Blending 53 Clipping 54
Hypocorisms 54 Backformation 54
Conversion 55 Coinage 56
Acronyms 56 Derivation 57
Prefixes and suffixes 57 Infixes 57
Multiple processes 58 Study questions 59 Tasks 60 Discussion topics/projects 62 Further reading 63
l6 Morphology Morphology 66 Morphemes 66 Free and bound morphemes 66
Lexical and functional morphemes 67 Derivational morphemes 67 Inflectional morphemes 68
Morphological description 68 Problems in morphological description 69
Morphs and allomorphs 69 Other languages 70
Kanuri 70 Ganda 71 Ilocano 71 Tagalog 71
Study questions 73 Tasks 73 Discussion topics/projects 76 Further reading 78
l7 Grammar English grammar 80 Traditional grammar 80
The parts of speech 80 Agreement 81 Grammatical gender 82 Traditional analysis 83
The prescriptive approach 83 Captain Kirk’s infinitive 84
The descriptive approach 84 Structural analysis 85 Constituent analysis 85 Labeled and bracketed sentences 86 Hierarchical organization 87 A Gaelic sentence 87 Why study grammar? 88
Study questions 89 Tasks 89 Discussion topics/projects 92 Further reading 93
l8 Syntax Syntactic rules 95
A generative grammar 95 Deep and surface structure 96
Structural ambiguity 96 Tree diagrams 97
Tree diagram of an English sentence 97
Symbols used in syntactic analysis 98 Phrase structure rules 99 Lexical rules 100 Movement rules 101 Study questions 103 Tasks 104 Discussion topics/projects 106 Further reading 108
vi Contents
l9 Semantics Meaning 110 Semantic features 110
Words as containers of meaning 111
Semantic roles 112 Agent and theme 112 Instrument and experiencer 112 Location, source and goal 113
Lexical relations 113 Synonymy 113 Antonymy 114 Hyponymy 115 Prototypes 116 Homophones and homonyms 116 Polysemy 117 Word play 117 Metonymy 118
Collocation 118 Study questions 120 Tasks 120 Discussion topics/projects 123 Further reading 123
l10 Pragmatics Pragmatics 126 Context 127
Deixis 128 Reference 128
Inference 129 Anaphora 129 Presupposition 130
Speech acts 131 Direct and indirect speech acts 131
Politeness 132 Negative and positive face 133
Study questions 134 Tasks 134 Discussion topics/projects 136 Further reading 138
l11 Discourse analysis Discourse 140
Interpreting discourse 140 Cohesion 141 Coherence 142 Speech events 142
Conversation analysis 143 Turn-taking 143
The co-operative principle 144 Hedges 145 Implicatures 146
Background knowledge 146 Schemas and scripts 147
Study questions 149 Tasks 149 Discussion topics/projects 151 Further reading 152
l12 Language and the brain Neurolinguistics 155 Language areas in the brain 155
Broca’s area 156 Wernicke’s area 156 The motor cortex and the arcuate fasciculus 157 The localization view 157
Tongue tips and slips 158 The tip of the tongue phenomenon 158 Slips of the tongue 158 Slips of the brain 159 Slips of the ear 159
Aphasia 160 Broca’s aphasia 160 Wernicke’s aphasia 160 Conduction aphasia 161
Dichotic listening 161 Left brain, right brain 162
The critical period 163 Genie 163
Study questions 165 Tasks 165 Discussion topics/projects 166 Further reading 167
Contents vii
l13 First language acquisition Acquisition 170
Input 170 Caregiver speech 171
The acquisition schedule 171 Cooing 172 Babbling 172 The one-word stage 173 The two-word stage 173 Telegraphic speech 174
The acquisition process 174 Learning through imitation? 175 Learning through correction? 175
Developing morphology 176 Developing syntax 177
Forming questions 177 Forming negatives 178
Developing semantics 178 Later developments 179
Study questions 181 Tasks 181 Discussion topics/projects 183 Further reading 184
l14 Second language acquisition/learning Second language learning 187
Acquisition and learning 187 Acquisition barriers 187 The age factor 188 Affective factors 188
Focus on teaching method 189 The grammar–translation method 189 The audiolingual method 190 Communicative approaches 190
Focus on the learner 190 Transfer 191 Interlanguage 191 Motivation 192 Input and output 192 Task-based learning 193
Communicative competence 194 Applied linguistics 194 Study questions 196 Tasks 196 Discussion topics/projects 198 Further reading 198
l15 Gestures and sign languages Gestures 201
Iconics 201 Deictics 201 Beats 202
Types of sign languages 202 Oralism 203 Signed English 203 Origins of ASL 204 The structure of signs 204
Shape and orientation 205 Location 205 Movement 205 Primes 205 Facial expressions and finger-spelling 206
The meaning of signs 206 Representing signs 207 ASL as a natural language 208 Study questions 209 Tasks 209 Discussion topics/projects 210 Further reading 210
l16 Written language Writing 213
Pictograms 213 Ideograms 213 Logograms 214
Phonographic writing 215 The rebus principle 216
Syllabic writing 216 Alphabetic writing 217 Written English 218
English orthography 219 Study questions 221 Tasks 221 Discussion topics/projects 223 Further reading 224
viii Contents
l17 Language history and change Family trees 227 Indo-European 227
Cognates 228 Comparative reconstruction 228
General principles 229 Sound reconstruction 229 Word reconstruction 230
The history of English 230 Old English 231 Middle English 231
Sound changes 232 Metathesis 233 Epenthesis 233 Prothesis 234
Syntactic changes 234 Loss of inflections 234
Semantic changes 235 Broadening of meaning 235 Narrowing of meaning 235
Diachronic and synchronic variation 236 Study questions 237 Tasks 237 Discussion topics/projects 239 Further reading 240
l18 Regional variation in language The standard language 243 Accent and dialect 243
Variation in grammar 244 Dialectology 244
Regional dialects 244 Isoglosses and dialect boundaries 245 The dialect continuum 246
Bilingualism 247 Diglossia 248
Language planning 249 Pidgins 250 Creoles 251
The post-creole continuum 251
Study questions 252 Tasks 252
Discussion topics/projects 254 Further reading 254
l19 Social variation in language Sociolinguistics 257
Social dialects 257 Education and occupation 257 Social markers 259
Speech style and style-shifting 259 Prestige 260
Speech accommodation 261 Convergence 261 Divergence 261
Register 261 Jargon 262
Slang 262 Taboo terms 263
African American English 263 Vernacular language 263 The sounds of a vernacular 264 The grammar of a vernacular 264
Study questions 266 Tasks 266 Discussion topics/projects 268 Further reading 268
l20 Language and culture Culture 271 Categories 271
Kinship terms 272 Time concepts 272
Linguistic relativity 273 The Sapir–Whorf hypothesis 273 Against the Sapir–Whorf hypothesis 274 Snow 274 Non-lexicalized categories 275
Cognitive categories 275 Classifiers 276
Social categories 276 Address terms 277
Gender 278 Gendered words 278
Contents ix
Gendered structures 279 Gendered speech 279 Same-gender talk 280 Gendered interaction 280
Study questions 281 Tasks 281
Discussion topics/projects 284 Further reading 284
Glossary 286 References 300 Index 312
x Contents
Preface
In this new edition
For all their advice and suggestions for improvements to the fifth edition of this book,
I’d like to thank the reviewers, instructors, students and researchers who have
commented on earlier versions. I have made a number of revisions in the internal
organization of all the chapters, with a clearer division into major topics and subsec-
tions. Additional section headings have been included to make the material more
accessible and a number of extra examples from everyday language use are offered
to make some of the points clearer. There are also more substantial revisions in
Chapters 3 (Phonetics), 4 (Phonology), 5 (Word formation) and 8 (Syntax) that
should make these units more manageable. I hope these revisions will make the book
more informative, easier to read, and overall more user-friendly.
In addition, there are thirty new tasks. The majority of these are data-based and
designed to foster problem-solving and critical-thinking skills. New examples from
languages as diverse as German, Hawaiian, Hungarian, Lakhota, Proto-Polynesian,
Quechua, Spanish and Tamasheq provide an opportunity to explore further aspects of
languages other than English. Additional topics explored in the study of the English
language include adjective order, adverb position in sentences, American and British
differences, compounds, general extenders, the presuppositions of jokes, recasts,
stylistics, synecdoche and vague language. An expanded and revised Study Guide
providing answers and tutorials for all the tasks can be found on the book’s website:
www.cambridge.org/yule.
To the student
In The Study of Language, I have tried to present a comprehensive survey of what is
known about language and also of the methods used by linguists in arriving at that
knowledge. There have been many interesting developments in the study of language
over the past two decades, but it is still a fact that any individual speaker of a language
has a more comprehensive “unconscious” knowledge of how language works than any
linguist has yet been able to describe. Consequently, as you read each of the following
chapters, take a critical view of the effectiveness of the descriptions, the analyses, and
the generalizations by measuring them against your own intuitions about how your
language works. By the end of the book, you should feel that you do know quite a lot
http://www.cambridge.org/yule
about both the internal structure of language (its form) and the varied uses of language
in human life (its function), and also that you are ready to ask more of the kinds of
questions that professional linguists ask when they conduct their research.
At the end of each chapter, there is a section where you can test and apply what
you have learned. This section contains:
� Study questions that you can use to check if you have understood some of the main points and important terms introduced during that chapter
� Tasks that extend the topics covered in the book, mostly through data analysis, with examples from English and a wide range of other languages
� Discussion topics/projects that offer opportunities to consider some of the more general, sometimes controversial, language-related topics and to develop your own
opinions on issues involving language
� Further reading suggestions provided to help you find more detailed treatments of all the topics covered in that chapter
The origins of this book can be traced to introductory courses on language taught at
the University of Edinburgh, the University of Minnesota and Louisiana State Univer-
sity, and to the suggestions and criticisms of hundreds of students who forced me to
present what I had to say in a way they could understand. An early version of the
written material was developed for Independent Study students at the University of
Minnesota. Later versions have had the benefit of expert advice from a lot of teachers
working with diverse groups in different situations. I am particularly indebted to
Professor Hugh Buckingham, Louisiana State University, for sharing his expertise
and enthusiasm over many years as a colleague and friend.
For feedback and advice in the preparation of recent editions of the book, I would
like to thank Jean Aitchison (University of Oxford), Linda Blanton (University of New
Orleans), Karen Currie (Federal University of Espı́ritu Santo), Mary Anna Dimitrako-
poulos (Indiana University, South Bend), Thomas Field (University of Maryland,
Baltimore), Anthony Fox (University of Leeds), Agustinus Gianto (Pontifical Biblical
Institute), Gordon Gibson (University of Paisley), Katinka Hammerich (University of
Hawai‘i), Raymond Hickey (University of Duisburg–Essen), Daniel Hieber (Rosetta
Stone), Richard Hirsch (Linköping University), Fiona Joseph (University of Wolver-
hampton), Eliza Kitis (Aristotle University), Terrie Mathis (California State University,
Northridge), Stephen Matthews (University of Hong Kong), Robyn Najar (Flinders
University), Eric Nelson (University of Minnesota), Jens Reinke (Christian Albrecht
University Kiel), Philip Riley (University of Nancy 2), Rick Santos (Fresno City
College), Joanne Scheibman (Old Dominion University), Royal Skousen (Brigham
Young University), Michael Stubbs (University of Trier), Mary Talbot (University of
Sunderland) and Sherman Wilcox (University of New Mexico).
For my own introductory course, I remain indebted to Willie and Annie Yule, and,
for my continuing enlightenment, to Maryann Overstreet.
xii Preface
CHAPTER 1
The origins of language
The suspicion does not appear improbable that the progenitors of man, either the males
or females, or both sexes, before they had acquired the power of expressing their mutual love
in articulate language, endeavoured to charm each other with musical notes and rhythm.
Darwin (1871)
In Charles Darwin’s vision of the origins of language, early humans had already
developed musical ability prior to language and were using it “to charm each other.”
This may not match the typical image that most of us have of our early ancestors as
rather rough characters wearing animal skins and not very charming, but it is an
interesting speculation about how language may have originated. It remains,
however, a speculation.
We simply don’t know how language originated. We do know that the ability
to produce sound and simple vocal patterning (a hum versus a grunt, for example)
appears to be in an ancient part of the brain that we share with all vertebrates,
including fish, frogs, birds and other mammals. But that isn’t human language. We
suspect that some type of spoken languagemust have developed between 100,000 and
50,000 years ago, well before written language (about 5,000 years ago). Yet, among
the traces of earlier periods of life on earth, we never find any direct evidence or
artifacts relating to the speech of our distant ancestors that might tell us how language
was back in the early stages. Perhaps because of this absence of direct physical evidence,
there has been no shortage of speculation about the origins of human speech.
The divine source
In the biblical tradition, as described in the book of Genesis, God created Adam and
“whatsoever Adam called every living creature, that was the name thereof.”Alternatively,
following a Hindu tradition, language came from Sarasvati, wife of Brahma, creator of the
universe. In most religions, there appears to be a divine source who provides humans
with language. In an attempt to rediscover this original divine language, a fewexperiments
have been carried out, with rather conflicting results. The basic hypothesis seems to have
been that, if human infantswere allowed to growupwithout hearing any language around
them, then they would spontaneously begin using the original God-given language.
The Greek writer Herodotus reported the story of an Egyptian pharaoh named
Psammetichus (or Psamtik) who tried the experiment with two newborn babies more
than 2,500 years ago. After two years of isolation except for the company of goats and a
mute shepherd, the children were reported to have spontaneously uttered, not an
Egyptian word, but something that was identified as the Phrygian word bekos, meaning
“bread.” The pharaoh concluded that Phrygian, an older language spoken in part ofwhat
is modern Turkey, must be the original language. That seems very unlikely. The children
may not have picked up this “word” from any human source, but as several commen-
tators have pointed out, they must have heard what the goats were saying. (First remove
the -kos ending,whichwas added in theGreek version of the story, then pronounce be- as
you would the English word bed without -d at the end. Can you hear a goat?)
King James the Fourth of Scotland carried out a similar experiment around the
year 1500 and the children were reported to have spontaneously started speaking
Hebrew, confirming the king’s belief that Hebrew had indeed been the language of the
Garden of Eden. It is unfortunate that all other cases of children who have been
discovered living in isolation, without coming into contact with human speech, tend
not to confirm the results of these types of divine-source experiments. Very young
children living without access to human language in their early years grow up with no
language at all. This was true of Victor, the wild boy of Aveyron in France, discovered
near the end of the eighteenth century, and also of Genie, an American child whose
special life circumstances came to light in the 1970s (see Chapter 12). From this type
of evidence, there is no “spontaneous” language. If human language did emanate
from a divine source, we have no way of reconstructing that original language,
especially given the events in a place called Babel, “because the Lord did there
confound the language of all the earth,” as described in Genesis (11: 9).
The natural sound source
A quite different view of the beginnings of language is based on the concept of natural
sounds. The human auditory system is already functioning before birth (at around
2 The Study of Language
seven months). That early processing capacity develops into an ability to identify
sounds in the environment, allowing humans to make a connection between a sound
and the thing producing that sound. This leads to the idea that primitive words derive
from imitations of the natural sounds that early men and women heard around them.
Among several nicknames that he invented to talk about the origins of speech,
Jespersen (1922) called this idea the “bow-wow” theory.
The “bow-wow” theory
In this scenario, when different objects flew by, making a Caw-Caw or Coo-Coo
sound, the early human tried to imitate the sounds and then used them to refer to
those objects even when they weren’t present. The fact that all modern languages
have some words with pronunciations that seem to echo naturally occurring sounds
could be used to support this theory. In English, in addition to cuckoo, we have splash,
bang, boom, rattle, buzz, hiss, screech, and of course bow-wow.
Words that sound similar to the noises they describe are examples of
onomatopeia. While it is true that a number of words in any language are onomato-
poeic, it is hard to see how most of the soundless things (e.g. “low branch”) as well as
abstract concepts (e.g. “truth”) could have been referred to in a language that simply
echoed natural sounds. We might also be rather skeptical about a view that seems to
assume that a language is only a set of words used as “names” for things.
The “pooh-pooh” theory
Another of Jespersen’s nicknames was the “pooh-pooh” theory, which proposed that
speech developed from the instinctive sounds people make in emotional circum-
stances. That is, the original sounds of language may have come from natural cries
of emotion such as pain, anger and joy. By this route, presumably, Ouch! came to have
its painful connotations. But Ouch! and other interjections such as Ah!, Ooh!, Phew!,
Wow! or Yuck! are usually produced with sudden intakes of breath, which is the
opposite of ordinary talk. We normally produce spoken language as we breath out, so
we speak while we exhale, not inhale. In other words, the expressive noises people
make in emotional reactions contain sounds that are not otherwise used in speech
production and consequently would seem to be rather unlikely candidates as source
sounds for language.
The social interaction source
Another proposal involving natural sounds was nicknamed the “yo-he-ho” theory.
The idea is that the sounds of a person involved in physical effort could be the source
of our language, especially when that physical effort involved several people and the
The origins of language 3
interaction had to be coordinated. So, a group of early humans might develop a set of
hums, grunts, groans and curses that were used when they were lifting and carrying
large bits of trees or lifeless hairy mammoths.
The appeal of this proposal is that it places the development of human language in
a social context. Early people must have lived in groups, if only because larger groups
offered better protection from attack. Groups are necessarily social organizations and,
to maintain those organizations, some form of communication is required, even if it is
just grunts and curses. So, human sounds, however they were produced, must have
had some principled use within the life and social interaction of early human groups.
This is an important idea that may relate to the uses of humanly produced sounds. It
does not, however, answer our question regarding the origins of the sounds produced.
Apes and other primates live in social groups and use grunts and social calls, but they
do not seem to have developed the capacity for speech.
The physical adaptation source
Instead of looking at types of sounds as the source of human speech, we can look at
the types of physical features humans possess, especially those that are distinct from
other creatures, which may have been able to support speech production. We can start
with the observation that, at some early stage, our ancestors made a very significant
transition to an upright posture, with bi-pedal (on two feet) locomotion, and a revised
role for the front limbs.
Some effects of this type of change can be seen in physical differences between the
skull of a gorilla and that of a Neanderthal man from around 60,000 years ago. The
reconstructed vocal tract of a Neanderthal suggests that some consonant-like sound
distinctions would have been possible. We have to wait until about 35,000 years ago
for features in reconstructions of fossilized skeletal structures that begin to resemble
those of modern humans. In the study of evolutionary development, there are certain
physical features, best thought of as partial adaptations, which appear to be relevant
for speech. They are streamlined versions of features found in other primates. By
themselves, such features wouldn’t guarantee speech, but they are good clues that a
creature with such features probably has the capacity for speech.
Teeth and lips
Human teeth are upright, not slanting outwards like those of apes, and they are
roughly even in height. Such characteristics are not very useful for ripping or tearing
food and seem better adapted for grinding and chewing. They are also very helpful in
making sounds such as f or v. Human lips have much more intricate muscle inter-
lacing than is found in other primates and their resulting flexibility certainly helps in
making sounds like p, b and m. In fact, the b and m sounds are the most widely
4 The Study of Language
attested in the vocalizations made by human infants during their first year, no matter
which language their parents are using.
Mouth and tongue
The human mouth is relatively small compared to other primates and can be opened
and closed rapidly. It is also part of an extended vocal tract that has much more of an
L-shape than the fairly straight path from front to back in other mammals. In contrast
to the fairly thin flat tongue of other large primates, humans have a shorter, thicker
and more muscular tongue that can be used to shape a wide variety of sounds inside
the oral cavity. In addition, unlike other primates, humans can close off the airway
through the nose to create more air pressure in the mouth. The overall effect of these
small differences taken together is a face with more intricate muscle interlacing in the
lips and mouth, capable of a wider range of shapes and a more rapid and powerful
delivery of sounds produced through these different shapes.
Larynx and pharynx
The human larynx or “voice box” (containing the vocal folds or vocal cords) differs
significantly in position from the larynx of other primates such as monkeys. In the
course of human physical development, the assumption of an upright posture moved
the head more directly above the spinal column and the larynx dropped to a lower
position. This created a longer cavity called the pharynx, above the vocal folds,
which acts as a resonator for increased range and clarity of the sounds produced via
the larynx and the vocal tract. Other primates have almost no pharynx. One unfortu-
nate consequence of this development is that the lower position of the human larynx
makes it much more possible for the human to choke on pieces of food. Monkeys
may not be able to use their larynx to produce speech sounds, but they do not suffer
from the problem of getting food stuck in their windpipe. In evolutionary terms, there
must have been a big advantage in getting this extra vocal power (i.e. a larger range
of sounds) to outweigh the potential disadvantage from an increased risk of choking
to death.
The tool-making source
In the physical adaptation view, one function (producing speech sounds) must have
been superimposed on existing anatomical features (teeth, lips) previously used for
other purposes (chewing, sucking). A similar development is believed to have taken
place with human hands and some believe that manual gestures may have been a
precursor of language. By about two million years ago, there is evidence that humans
had developed preferential right-handedness and had become capable of making
The origins of language 5
stone tools. Wood tools and composite tools eventually followed. Tool-making, or the
outcome of manipulating objects and changing them using both hands, is evidence of
a brain at work.
The human brain
The human brain is not only large relative to human body size, it is also lateralized,
that is, it has specialized functions in each of the two hemispheres. (More details are
presented in Chapter 12.) Those functions that control the motor movements involved
in complex vocalization (speaking) and object manipulation (making or using tools)
are very close to each other in the left hemisphere of the brain. That is, the area of the
motor cortex that controls the muscles of the arms and hands is next to the articulatory
muscles of the face, jaw and tongue. It may be that there was an evolutionary connec-
tion between the language-using and tool-using abilities of humans and that both were
involved in the development of the speaking brain. Most of the other speculative
proposals concerning the origins of speech seem to be based on a picture of humans
producing single noises to indicate objects in their environment. This activity may
indeed have been a crucial stage in the development of language, but what it lacks is
any structural organization. All languages, including sign language, require the organ-
izing and combining of sounds or signs in specific arrangements. We seem to have
developed a part of our brain that specializes in making these arrangements.
If we think in terms of the most basic process involved in primitive tool-making, it
is not enough to be able to grasp one rock (make one sound); the human must also be
able to bring another rock (other sounds) into proper contact with the first in order to
develop a tool. In terms of language structure, the human may have first developed a
naming ability by producing a specific and consistent noise (e.g. beer) for a specific
object. The crucial additional step was to bring another specific noise (e.g. good) into
combination with the first to build a complex message (beer good). Several thousand
years of development later, humans have honed this message-building capacity to a
point where, on Saturdays, watching a football game, they can drink a sustaining
beverage and proclaim This beer is good. As far as we know, other primates are not
doing this.
The genetic source
We can think of the human baby in its first few years as a living example of some of
these physical changes taking place. At birth, the baby’s brain is only a quarter of its
eventual weight and the larynx is much higher in the throat, allowing babies, like
chimpanzees, to breathe and drink at the same time. In a relatively short period of
time, the larynx descends, the brain develops, the child assumes an upright posture
and starts walking and talking.
6 The Study of Language
This almost automatic set of developments and the complexity of the young
child’s language have led some scholars to look for something more powerful than
small physical adaptations of the species over time as the source of language. Even
children who are born deaf (and do not develop speech) become fluent sign language
users, given appropriate circumstances, very early in life. This seems to indicate that
human offspring are born with a special capacity for language. It is innate, no other
creature seems to have it, and it isn’t tied to a specific variety of language. Is it
possible that this language capacity is genetically hard-wired in the newborn human?
As a solution to the puzzle of the origins of language, this innateness hypothesis
would seem to point to something in human genetics, possibly a crucial mutation, as
the source. This would not have been a gradual change, but something that happened
rather quickly. We are not sure when this proposed genetic change might have taken
place or how it might relate to the physical adaptations described earlier. However, as
we consider this hypothesis, we find our speculations about the origins of language
moving away from fossil evidence or the physical source of basic human sounds
toward analogies with how computers work (e.g. being pre-programmed or hard-
wired) and concepts taken from the study of genetics. The investigation of the origins
of language then turns into a search for the special “language gene” that only humans
possess.
If we are indeed the only creatures with this special capacity for language, then
will it be completely impossible for any other creature to produce or understand
language? We’ll try to answer that question in Chapter 2.
The origins of language 7
STUDY QUESTIONS
1 Why are interjections such as Ooh! or Yuck! considered to be unlikely sources of
human speech sounds?
2 What is the basic idea behind the “bow-wow” theory of language origin?
3 Why is it difficult to agree with Psammetichus that Phrygian must have been the
original human language?
4 Where is the pharynx and how did it become an important part of human sound
production?
5 Why do you think that young deaf children who become fluent in sign language
would be cited in support of the innateness hypothesis?
6 With which of the six “sources” would you associate this quotation?
Chewing, licking and sucking are extremely widespread mammalian activities,
which, in terms of casual observation, have obvious similarities with speech.
(MacNeilage, 1998)
TASKS
A What is the connection between the Heimlich maneuver and the development of
human speech?
B What exactly happened at Babel and why is it used in explanations of language
origins?
C What are the arguments for and against a teleological explanation of the origins of
human language?
D The idea that “ontogeny recapitulates phylogeny” was first proposed
by Ernst Haeckel in 1866 and is still frequently used in discussions of
language origins. Can you find a simpler or less technical way to express
this idea?
E The Danish linguist Otto Jespersen, who gave us the terms “bow-wow” and “pooh-
pooh” for theories about language origins, dismissed both of these ideas in favor of
another theory. What explanation did Jespersen (1922, chapter 21) favor as the
likely origin of early speech?
F In his analysis of the beginnings of human language, William Foley comes to the
conclusion that “language as we understand it was born about 200,000 years ago”
(1997: 73). This is substantially earlier than the dates (between 100,000 and 50,000
years ago) that other scholars have proposed. What kinds of evidence and
arguments are typically presented in order to choose a particular date “when
language was born”?
8 The Study of Language
G What is the connection between the innateness hypothesis, as described in this
chapter, and the idea of a Universal Grammar?
H When it was first identified, the FOXP2 gene was hailed as the “language gene.”
What was the basis of this claim and how has it been modified?
DISCUSSION TOPICS/PROJECTS
I In this chapter we didn’t address the issue of whether language has developed as
part of our general cognitive abilities or whether it has evolved as a separate
component that can exist independently (and is unrelated to intelligence, for
example). What kind of evidence do you think would be needed to resolve this
question?
(For background reading, see chapter 4 of Aitchison, 2000.)
II A connection has been proposed between language, tool-using and right-
handedness in the majority of humans. Is it possible that freedom to use the hands,
after assuming an upright bipedal posture, resulted in certain skills that led to the
development of language? Why did we assume an upright posture? What kind of
changes must have taken place in our hands?
(For background reading, see Beaken, 2011.)
FURTHER READING
Basic treatments
Aitchison, J. (2000) The Seeds of Speech (Canto edition) Cambridge University Press
Kenneally, C. (2007) The First Word Viking Press
More detailed treatments
Beaken, M. (2011) The Making of Language (2nd edition) Dunedin Academic Press
McMahon, A. and R. McMahon (2013) Evolutionary Linguistics Cambridge
University Press
Music before language
Mithen, S. (2006) The Singing Neanderthals Harvard University Press
A hum versus a grunt
Bass, A., E. Gilland and R. Baker (2008) “Evolutionary origins for social vocalization in a
vertebrate hindbrain-spinal compartment” Science 321 (July 18): 417–421
Victor and Genie
Lane, H. (1976) The Wild Boy of Aveyron Harvard University Press
Rymer, R. (1993) Genie HarperCollins
“Bow-wow” theory, etc.
Jespersen, O. (1922) Language: Its Nature, Development and Origin George Allen & Unwin
The early sounds made by infants
Locke, J. (1983) Phonological Acquisition and Change Academic Press
The origins of language 9
Social interaction
Burling, R. (2005) The Talking Ape Oxford University Press
Physical development
Lieberman, P. (1998) Eve Spoke: Human Language and Human Evolution W. W. Norton
Gesture
Corballis, M. (2002) From Hand to Mouth Princeton University Press
McNeill, D. (2012) How Language Began: Gesture and Speech in Human Evolution Cambridge
University Press
Brain development
Loritz, D. (1999) How the Brain Evolved Language Oxford University Press
Tool-making
Gibson, K. and T. Ingold (eds.) (1993) Tools, Language and Cognition in Human Evolution
Cambridge University Press
Innateness
Pinker, S. (1994) The Language Instinct William Morrow
Against innateness
Sampson, G. (2005) The “Language Instinct” Debate (revised edition) Continuum
Other references
Foley, W. (1997) Anthropological Linguistics Blackwell
MacNeilage, P. (1998) “The frame/content theory of evolution of speech production”
Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 499–546
10 The Study of Language
CHAPTER 2
Animals and human language
One evening in the mid-1980s my wife and I were returning from an evening cruise around
Boston Harbor and decided to take a waterfront stroll. We were passing in front of the Boston
Aquarium when a gravelly voice yelled out, “Hey! Hey! Get outa there!” Thinking we had
mistakenly wandered somewhere we were not allowed, we stopped and looked around for a
security guard or some other official, but saw no one, and no warning signs. Again the voice
boomed, “Hey! Hey you!” As we tracked the voice we found ourselves approaching a large,
glass-fenced pool in front of the aquarium where four harbor seals were lounging on display.
Incredulous, I traced the source of the command to a large seal reclining vertically in the water,
with his head extended back and up, his mouth slightly open, rotating slowly. A seal was
talking, not to me, but to the air, and incidentally to anyone within earshot who cared to listen.
Deacon (1997)
There are a lot of stories about creatures that can talk. We usually assume that
they are fantasy or fiction or that they involve birds or animals simply imitating
something they have heard humans say (as Terrence Deacon discovered was the case
with the loud seal in Boston Aquarium). Yet we think that creatures are capable of
communicating, certainly with other members of their own species. Is it possible that
a creature could learn to communicate with humans using language? Or does human
language have properties that make it so unique that it is quite unlike any other
communication system and hence unlearnable by any other creature? To answer
these questions, we first look at some special properties of human language, then
review a number of experiments in communication involving humans and animals.
Communication
We should first distinguish between specifically communicative signals and those
which may be unintentionally informative signals. Someone listening to you may
become informed about you through a number of signals that you have not intention-
ally sent. She may note that you have a cold (you sneezed), that you aren’t at ease
(you shifted around in your seat), that you are disorganized (non-matching socks)
and that you are from somewhere else (you have a strange accent). However, when
you use language to tell this person, I’m one of the applicants for the vacant position of
senior brain surgeon at the hospital, you are normally considered to be intentionally
communicating something.
Humans are capable of producing sounds and syllables in a stream of speech
that appears to have no communicative purpose, as in glossolalia, or “speaking in
tongues,” which is associated with the religious practices of Pentecostal churches.
These outpourings sound like language, but with no speaker control, it isn’t inten-
tional communication. We might say the same thing about some of the chirping and
singing produced by birds. We also don’t assume that the blackbird is communi-
cating anything by having black feathers, sitting on a branch and looking down at
the ground. However, the bird is considered to be sending a communicative signal
with the loud squawking that is produced when a cat appears on the scene.
So, when we talk about distinctions between human language and animal
communication, we are considering both in terms of their potential as a means of
intentional communication.
Properties of human language
While we tend to think of communication as the primary function of human language,
it is not a distinguishing feature. All creatures communicate in some way, even if it’s
not through vocalization. However, we suspect that other creatures are not reflecting
on the way they create their communicative messages or reviewing how they work (or
not). That is, one barking dog is probably not offering advice to another barking dog
along the lines of “Hey, you should lower your bark to make it sound more men-
acing.” They’re not barking about barking. Humans are clearly able to reflect on
language and its uses (e.g. “I wish he wouldn’t use so many technical terms”). This is
reflexivity. The property of reflexivity (or “reflexiveness”) accounts for the fact that
we can use language to think and talk about language itself, making it one of the
distinguishing features of human language. Indeed, without this general ability, we
wouldn’t be able to reflect on or identify any of the other distinct properties of human
language. We’ll look in detail at another five of them: displacement, arbitrariness,
productivity, cultural transmission and duality.
12 The Study of Language
Displacement
When your pet cat comes home and stands at your feet callingmeow, you are likely to
understand this message as relating to that immediate time and place. If you ask your
cat what it’s been up to, you’ll probably get the same meow response. Animal
communication seems to be designed exclusively for this moment, here and now. It
isn’t used to relate events that are far removed in time and place. When your dog says
GRRR, it means GRRR, right now, because dogs aren’t capable of communicating
GRRR, last night, over in the park. In contrast, human language users are normally
capable of producing messages equivalent to GRRR, last night, over in the park, and
then going on to say In fact, I’ll be going back tomorrow for some more. Humans can
refer to past and future time. This property of human language is called
displacement. It allows language users to talk about things and events not present
in the immediate environment. Indeed, displacement allows us to talk about things
and places (e.g. angels, fairies, Santa Claus, Superman, heaven, hell) whose existence
we cannot even be sure of.
We could look at bee communication as a small exception because it seems to
have some version of displacement. When a honeybee finds a source of nectar
and returns to the beehive, it can perform a dance routine to communicate to the
other bees the location of this nectar. Depending on the type of dance (round
dance for nearby and tail-wagging dance, with variable tempo, for further away
and how far), the other bees can work out where this newly discovered feast can
be found. Doesn’t this ability of the bee to indicate a location some distance away
mean that bee communication has at least some degree of displacement as a
feature? Yes, but it is displacement of a very limited type. It just doesn’t have
the range of possibilities found in human language. Certainly, the bee can direct
other bees to a food source. However, it must be the most recent food source.
It cannot be that delicious rose bush on the other side of town that we visited
last weekend, nor can it be, as far as we know, possible future nectar in
bee heaven.
Arbitrariness
It is generally the case that there is no “natural” connection between a linguistic form
and its meaning. The connection is quite arbitrary. We can’t just look at the Arabic
word کلب and, from its shape, for example, determine that it has a natural and
obvious meaning any more than we can with its English translation form dog. The
linguistic form has no natural or “iconic” relationship with that hairy four-legged
barking object out in the world. This aspect of the relationship between words and
objects is described as arbitrariness. It is possible, as in a child’s game, to make
words appear to “fit” the idea or activity they indicate, as shown in Figure 2.1.
Animals and human language 13
However, this type of game only emphasizes the arbitrariness of the connection that
normally exists between a word and its meaning.
There are some words in language with sounds that seem to “echo” the sounds of
objects or activities and hence seem to have a less arbitrary connection. English
examples are cuckoo, crash, slurp, squelch or whirr. However, these onomatopoeic
words are relatively rare in human language.
For the majority of animal signals, there does appear to be a clear connection
between the conveyed message and the signal used to convey it. This impression may
be closely connected to the fact that, for any animal, the set of signals used in
communication is finite. Each variety of animal communication consists of a limited
set of vocal or gestural forms. Many of these forms are only used in specific situations
(to establish territory) or at particular times (to find a mate).
Productivity
Humans are continually creating new expressions by manipulating their linguistic
resources to describe new objects and situations. This property is described as
productivity (or “creativity” or “open-endedness”) and essentially means that the
potential number of utterances in any human language is infinite.
The communication systems of other creatures are not like that. Cicadas have
four signals to choose from and vervet monkeys have thirty-six vocal calls. Nor
does it seem possible for creatures to produce new signals to communicate novel
experiences or events. The honeybee, normally able to communicate the location of
a nectar source to other bees, will fail to do so if the location is really “new.” In one
experiment, a hive of bees was placed at the foot of a radio tower and a food source
placed at the top. Ten bees were taken to the top, given a taste of the delicious food,
and sent off to tell the rest of the hive about their find. The message was conveyed
via a bee dance and the whole gang buzzed off to get the free food. They flew
around in all directions, but couldn’t locate the food. (It’s probably one way to
make bees really mad.) The problem seems to be that bee communication has a
fixed set of signals for communicating location and they all relate to horizontal
distance. The bee cannot manipulate its communication system to create a “new”
message indicating vertical distance. According to Karl von Frisch, who conducted
the experiment, “the bees have no word for up in their language” and they can’t
invent one.
Figure 2.1
14 The Study of Language
This lack of productivity in animal communication can be described in terms of
fixed reference. Each signal in the communication system of other creatures seems to
be fixed in terms of relating to a particular occasion or purpose. This is particularly
true of scent-based signaling, as in the pheromones (a chemical substance) released
by insects such as female moths as they try to contact a mate. It’s a case of one scent,
one meaning.
Among our closer relatives, there are lemurs (similar to small monkeys) in
Madagascar that have only three basic calls, each tied to one type of dangerous or
threatening situation. In the vervet monkey’s repertoire, there is one danger signal
CHUTTER, which is used when a snake is around, and another RRAUP, used when an
eagle is spotted nearby. These signals are fixed in terms of their reference and cannot
be manipulated. What might be presented as evidence of productivity in the monkey’s
communication system would be an utterance of something like CHUTT-RRAUP when
a flying creature that looked like a snake came by. Despite a lot of research involving
snakes suddenly appearing in the air above them (among other unusual and terrifying
experiences), the vervet monkeys didn’t produce a new danger signal. The human,
given similar circumstances, is quite capable of creating a “new” signal, after initial
surprise perhaps, by saying something never said before, as in Hey! Watch out for that
flying snake!
Cultural transmission
While we may inherit physical features such as brown eyes and dark hair from our
parents, we do not inherit their language. We acquire a language in a culture with
other speakers and not from parental genes. An infant born to Korean parents in
Korea, but adopted and brought up from birth by English speakers in the United
States, will have physical characteristics inherited from his or her natural parents, but
will inevitably speak English. A kitten, given comparable early experiences, will
produce meow regardless.
This process whereby a language is passed on from one generation to the next is
described as cultural transmission. It is clear that humans are born with some kind
of predisposition to acquire language in a general sense. However, we are not born
with the ability to produce utterances in a specific language such as English. We
acquire our first language as children in a culture.
The general pattern in animal communication is that creatures are born with a set
of specific signals that are produced instinctively. There is some evidence from studies
of birds as they develop their songs that instinct has to combine with learning (or
exposure) in order for the right song to be produced. If those birds spend their first
seven weeks without hearing other birds, they will instinctively produce songs or
calls, but those songs will be abnormal in some way. Human infants, growing up in
isolation, produce no “instinctive” language.
Animals and human language 15
Duality
Human language is organized at two levels or layers simultaneously. This property is
called duality (or “double articulation”). When we speak, we have a physical level at
which we produce individual sounds, like n, b and i. As individual sounds, none of
these discrete forms has any intrinsic meaning. In a particular combination such as
bin, we have another level producing a meaning that is different from the meaning of
the combination in nib. So, at one level, we have distinct sounds, and, at another
level, we have distinct meanings. This duality of levels is one of the most economical
features of human language because, with a limited set of discrete sounds, we are
capable of producing a very large number of sound combinations (e.g. words) that are
distinct in meaning.
Among other creatures, each communicative signal appears to be a single fixed
form that cannot be broken down into separate parts. Although your dog may be able
to produce woof (“I’m happy to see you”), it does not seem to do so on the basis of a
distinct level of production combining the separate elements of w þ oo þ f. If the dog was operating with the double level (i.e. duality), then we might expect to hear
different combinations with different meanings, such as oowf (“I’m hungry”) and
foow (“I’m really bored”).
Talking to animals
If these properties make human language such a unique communication system, then
it would seem extremely unlikely that other creatures would be able to understand it.
Some humans, however, do not behave as if this is the case. There is a lot of spoken
language directed by humans to animals, apparently under the impression that the
animal follows what is being said. Riders can say Whoa to horses and they stop, we
can say Heel to dogs and they will follow at heel (well, sometimes), and a variety of
circus animals go Up, Down and Roll over in response to spoken commands. Should
we treat these examples as evidence that non-humans can understand human lan-
guage? Probably not. The standard explanation is that the animal produces a particu-
lar behavior in response to a particular sound stimulus, but does not actually
“understand” what the words in the noise mean.
If it seems difficult to conceive of animals understanding human language,
then it appears to be even less likely that an animal would be capable of produ-
cing human language. After all, we do not generally observe animals of one
species learning to produce the signals of another species. You could keep your
horse in a field of cows for years, but it still won’t say Moo. And, in some homes,
a new baby and a puppy may arrive at the same time. Baby and puppy grow up in
the same environment, hearing the same things, but two years later, the baby is
16 The Study of Language
making lots of human speech sounds and the puppy is not. Perhaps a puppy is a
poor example. Wouldn’t it be better to work with a closer relative such as a
chimpanzee?
Chimpanzees and language
The idea of raising a chimp and a child together may seem like a nightmare, but this is
basically what was done in an early attempt to teach a chimpanzee to use human
language. In the 1930s, two scientists (Luella and Winthrop Kellogg) reported on their
experience of raising an infant chimpanzee together with their baby son. The chim-
panzee, called Gua, was reported to be able to understand about a hundred words, but
did not “say” any of them. In the 1940s, a chimpanzee named Viki was reared by
another scientist couple (Catherine and Keith Hayes) in their own home, exactly as if
she was a human child. These foster parents spent five years attempting to get Viki to
“say” English words by trying to shape her mouth as she produced sounds. Viki
eventually managed to produce some words, rather poorly articulated versions of
mama, papa and cup. In retrospect, this was a remarkable achievement since it has
become clear that non-human primates do not actually have a physically structured
vocal tract which is suitable for articulating the sounds used in speech. Apes and
gorillas can, like chimpanzees, communicate with a wide range of vocal calls, but
they just can’t make human speech sounds.
Washoe
Recognizing that a chimpanzee was not likely to learn spoken language,
another scientist couple (Beatrix and Allen Gardner) set out to teach a female
chimpanzee called Washoe to use a version of American Sign Language. As
described later in Chapter 15, this sign language has all the essential properties
of human language and is learned by many congenitally deaf children as their
natural first language.
From the beginning, the Gardners and their research assistants raised Washoe like
a human child in a comfortable domestic environment. Sign language was always
used when Washoe was around and she was encouraged to use signs, even her own
incomplete “baby-versions” of the signs used by adults. In a period of three and a half
years, Washoe came to use signs for more than a hundred words, ranging from
airplane, baby and banana through to window, woman and you. Even more impres-
sive was Washoe’s ability to take these forms and combine them to produce “sen-
tences” of the type gimme tickle, more fruit and open food drink (to get someone to
open the refrigerator). Some of the forms appear to have been inventions by Washoe,
as in her novel sign for bib and in the combination water bird (referring to a swan),
Animals and human language 17
which would seem to indicate that her communication system had the potential for
productivity. Washoe also demonstrated understanding of a much larger number of
signs than she produced and was capable of holding rudimentary conversations,
mainly in the form of question–answer sequences. A similar ability with sign
language was reported by Francine Patterson working with a gorilla named Koko
not long after.
Sarah
At the same time as Washoe was learning sign language, another chimpanzee was
being taught (by Ann and David Premack) to use a set of plastic shapes for the
purpose of communicating with humans. These plastic shapes represented “words”
that could be arranged in sequence to build “sentences” (Sarah preferred a vertical
order, as shown in Figure 2.2). The basic approach was quite different from that of
the Gardners. Sarah was systematically trained to associate these shapes with
objects or actions. She remained an animal in a cage, being trained with food
rewards to manipulate a set of symbols. Once she had learned to use a large number
of these plastic shapes, Sarah was capable of getting an apple by selecting the correct
plastic shape (a blue triangle) from a large array. Notice that this symbol is arbitrary
since it would be hard to argue for any natural connection between an apple and a
blue plastic triangle. Sarah was also capable of producing “sentences” such as Mary
give chocolate Sarah and had the impressive capacity to understand complex struc-
tures such as If Sarah put red on green, Mary give Sarah chocolate. Sarah got the
chocolate.
MARY
GIVE
CHOCOLATE
SARAH
Figure 2.2
18 The Study of Language
Lana
A similar training technique with another artificial language was used (by Duane
Rumbaugh) to train a chimpanzee called Lana. The language she learned was called
Yerkish and consisted of a set of symbols on a large keyboard linked to a computer.
When Lana wanted some water, she had to find and press four symbols to produce the
message please machine give water, as illustrated in Figure 2.3.
Both Sarah and Lana demonstrated an ability to use what look like word symbols
and basic structures in ways that superficially resemble the use of language. There is,
however, a lot of skepticism regarding these apparent linguistic skills. It has been
pointed out that when Lana used the symbol for “please,” she did not have to
understand the meaning of the English word please. The symbol for “please” on the
computer keyboard might simply be the equivalent of a button on a vending machine
and, so the argument goes, we could learn to operate vending machines without
necessarily knowing language. This is only one of the many arguments that have been
presented against the idea that the use of signs and symbols by these chimpanzees is
similar to the use of language.
The controversy
On the basis of his work with another chimpanzee called Nim, the psychologist
Herbert Terrace argued that chimpanzees simply produce signs in response to the
demands of people and tend to repeat signs those people use, yet they are treated (by
naive researchers) as if they are taking part in a “conversation.” As in many critical
studies of animal learning, the chimpanzees’ behavior is viewed as a type of condi-
tioned response to cues provided (often unwittingly) by human trainers. Herbert’s
conclusion was that chimpanzees are clever creatures who learn to produce a certain
type of behavior (signing or symbol selection) in order to get rewards and are
essentially performing sophisticated “tricks.”
In response, the Gardners argued that they were not animal trainers, nor were
they inculcating and then eliciting conditioned responses from Washoe. In complex
experiments, designed to eliminate any possible provision of cues by humans, they
showed that in the absence of any human, Washoe could produce correct signs to
identify objects in pictures. They also emphasize a major difference between the
experiences of Washoe and Nim. While Nim was a research animal in a complex
Figure 2.3
Animals and human language 19
environment, having to deal with a lot of different research assistants who were often
not fluent in American Sign Language, Washoe lived in a more limited domestic
environment with a lot of opportunity for imaginative play and interaction with fluent
signers who were also using sign language with each other. They also report that
another group of younger chimpanzees not only learned sign language, but also
occasionally used signs with each other and with Washoe, even when there were no
humans present.
Kanzi
In a more recent set of studies, an interesting development relevant to this controversy
came about almost by accident. While Sue Savage-Rumbaugh was attempting to train
a bonobo (a pygmy chimpanzee) called Matata how to use the symbols of Yerkish,
Matata’s adopted baby, Kanzi, was always with her. Although Matata did not do very
well, her son Kanzi spontaneously started using the symbol system with great ease.
He had learned not by being taught, but by being exposed to, and observing, a kind of
language in use at a very early age. Kanzi eventually developed a large symbol
vocabulary (over 250 forms). By the age of eight, he was reported to be able to
demonstrate understanding of spoken English at a level comparable to a two-and-a-
half-year-old human child. There was also evidence that he was using a consistently
distinct set of “gentle noises” as words to refer to things such as bananas, grapes and
juice. He had also become capable of using his symbol system to ask to watch his
favorite movies, Quest for Fire (about primitive humans) and Greystoke (about the
Tarzan legend).
Using language
Important lessons have been learned from attempts to teach chimpanzees how to use
forms of language. We have answered some questions. Were Washoe and Kanzi
capable of taking part in interaction with humans by using a symbol system chosen
by humans and not chimpanzees? The answer is clearly “Yes.” Did Washoe and Kanzi
go on to perform linguistically on a level comparable to a human child about to begin
pre-school? The answer is just as clearly “No.” In arriving at these answers, we’ve had
to face the fact that, even with a list of key properties, we still don’t seem to have a
non-controversial definition of what “using language” means.
One solution might be to stop thinking of language, at least in the phrase “using
language,” as a single thing that one can either have or not have. We could then say
there are (at least) two ways of thinking about what “using language” means. In a
very broad sense, language serves as a type of communication system that can
be observed in different situations. In one situation, we look at the behavior of a
two-year-old human child interacting with a caregiver as an example of “using
20 The Study of Language
language.” In another situation, we observe very similar behavior from chimpanzees
and bonobos when they are interacting with humans they know. It has to be fair to say
that, in both cases, we observe the participants “using language.”
However, there is a difference. Underlying the two-year-old’s communicative
activity is the capacity to develop a complex system of sounds and structures, plus
computational procedures, that will allow the child to produce extended discourse
containing a potentially infinite number of novel utterances. No other creature has
been observed “using language” in this sense. It is in this more comprehensive and
productive sense that we say that language is uniquely human.
Animals and human language 21
STUDY QUESTIONS
1 What is the difference between a communication system with productivity and one
with fixed reference?
2 Why is reflexivity considered to be a special property of human language?
3 What kind of evidence is used to support the idea that language is culturally
transmitted?
4 How did the Gardners try to show that Washoe was not simply repeating signs
made by interacting humans?
5 If Sarah could use a gray plastic shape to convey the meaning of the word red,
which property does her “language” seem to have?
6 What was considered to be the key element in Kanzi’s language learning?
TASKS
A In studies of communication involving animals and humans, there is
sometimes a reference to “the Clever Hans phenomenon.” Who or what was
Clever Hans, why was he/she/it famous and what exactly is the
“phenomenon”?
B We recognized a distinction early in the chapter between communicative and
informative signals. How would “body language” be characterized? Also, what
kind of signaling is involved in “distance zones”? What about “eye contact” and
“eyebrow flashes”?
C What is meant by “sound symbolism” and how does it relate to the property of
arbitrariness?
D (i) In the study of animal communication, what are “playback experiments”?
(ii) Which forms of animal communication described in this chapter were
discovered as a result of playback experiments?
E What was the significance of the name given to the chimpanzee in the research
conducted by the psychologist Herbert Terrace (1979)?
F We reviewed studies involving chimpanzees and bonobos learning to
communicate with humans. Can only African apes accomplish this task? Are there
any studies involving the Asian great ape, the orangutan, learning how to use a
human communication system?
G Consider these statements about the symbol-using abilities of chimpanzees in
animal language studies and decide if they are correct or not. What evidence can be
used to argue for or against the accuracy of these statements?
22 The Study of Language
(1) They can create combinations of signs that look like the telegraphic speech
produced by young children.
(2) They can invent new sign combinations.
(3) They can understand structures with complex word order, such as conditionals
(i.e. if X, then Y).
(4) They overgeneralize the references of signs, using one sign for many different
things, just as human children do in the early stages.
(5) They don’t use signs spontaneously and only produce them in response to
humans.
(6) They have complex concepts such as time because they produce sign
combinations such as time eat.
(7) They use signs to interact with each other, just as three-year-old children do
with speech.
(8) They steadily increase the length of their utterances, so that their average
utterance length of 3.0 is equivalent to that of a three-and-a-half-year-old child.
H It has been claimed that “recursion” is a key property of human language, and
of human cognition in general. What is recursion? Could it still be a universal
property of human language if one language was discovered that had no evidence
of recursion in its structure?
DISCUSSION TOPICS/PROJECTS
I Listed below are six other properties (or “design features”) that are often discussed
when human language is compared to other communication systems.
vocal–auditory channel use (language signals are sent using the vocal organs and
received by the ears)
specialization (language signals do not serve any other type of purpose such as
breathing or feeding)
non-directionality (language signals have no inherent direction and can be picked
up by anyone within hearing, even unseen)
rapid fade (language signals are produced and disappear quickly)
reciprocity (any sender of a language signal can also be a receiver)
prevarication (language signals can be false or used to lie or deceive)
(i) Are these properties found in all forms of human communication via
language?
(ii) Are these special properties of human language or can they be found in the
communication systems of other creatures?
(For background reading, see chapter 18 of O’Grady et al., 2009)
Animals and human language 23
II Themost persistent criticism of the chimpanzee language-learning projects is that the
chimpanzees are simply making responses like trained animals for rewards and are
consequently not using language to express anything. Read over the following reports
and try to decide how the different behaviors of these chimpanzees (Dar,Washoe and
Moja) should be characterized. Signs are represented by words in capital letters.
After her nap, Washoe signed OUT. I was hoping for Washoe to potty herself and
did not comply. ThenWashoe took my hands and put them together to make OUT
and then signed OUT with her own hands to show me how.
Greg was hooting and making other sounds, to prevent Dar from falling asleep.
Dar put his fist to Greg’s lips and made kissing sounds. Greg asked WHAT
WANT? and Dar replied QUIET, placing the sign on Greg’s lips.
Moja signed DOG on Ron and me and looked at our faces, waiting for us to
“woof.” After several rounds I made a “meeow” instead. Moja signed DOG again,
I repeated “meeow” again, and Moja slapped my leg harder. This went on. Finally
I woofed and Moja leapt on me and hugged me.
Moja stares longingly at Dairy Queen as we drive by. Then for a minute or more
signs NO ICE CREAM many times, by shaking her head while holding fist to
mouth, index edge up.
(For background reading, see Rimpau et al., 1989, which is the source of these
examples. There is also a film with the title Project Nim (Lionsgate), that describes
the unfortunate experiences of the chimpanzee Nim.)
FURTHER READING
Basic treatments
Aitchison, J. (2011) The Articulate Mammal (chapter 2) Routledge Classics
Friend, T. (2005) Animal Talk Simon and Schuster
More detailed treatments
Anderson, S. (2004) Doctor Doolittle’s Delusion Yale University Press
Rogers, L. and G. Kaplan (2000) Songs, Roars and Rituals Harvard University Press
General properties of language
Hockett, C. (1960) “The origin of speech” Scientific American 203: 89–96
Glossolalia
Newberg, A., N. Wintering, D. Morgan and M. Waldman (2006). “The measurement of regional
cerebral blood flow during glossolalia: a preliminary SPECT study” Psychiatry Research:
Neuroimaging 148: 67–71
Samarin,W. (1972)TonguesofMenandAngels:TheReligiousLanguageof PentecostalismMacmillan
Animal communication and consciousness
Griffin, D. (2001) Animal Minds University of Chicago Press
Hauser, M. (1996) The Evolution of Communication MIT Press
24 The Study of Language
Bee communication
von Frisch, K. (1993) The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees Harvard University Press
Lemur and vervet monkey communication
Cheney, D. and R. Seyfarth (1990) How Monkeys See the World University of Chicago Press
Jolly, A. (1966) Lemur Behavior University of Chicago Press
Individual chimpanzees, gorillas and bonobos
(Gua) Kellogg, W. and L. Kellog (1933) The Ape and the Child McGraw-Hill
(Viki) Hayes, C. (1951) The Ape in Our House Harper
(Washoe) Gardner, R., B. Gardner and T. van Cantfort (eds.) (1989) Teaching Sign Language
to Chimpanzees State University of New York Press
(Koko) Patterson, F. and E. Linden (1981) The Education of Koko Holt
(Sarah) Premack, A. and D. Premack (1991) “Teaching language to an ape” In W. Wang (ed.)
The Emergence of Language (16–27) W. H. Freeman
(Lana) Rumbaugh, D. (ed.) (1977) Language Learning by a Chimpanzee: The LANA Project
Academic Press
(Nim) Hess, E. (2008) Nim Chimpsky: The Chimp Who Would Be Human Bantam Books
(Kanzi) Savage-Rumbaugh, S. and R. Lewin (1994) Kanzi: The Ape at the Brink of the Human
Mind John Wiley
Other references
O’Grady, W., J. Archibald, M. Aronoff and J. Rees-Miller (2009) Contemporary Linguistics (6th
edition) Bedford/St. Martins Press
Rimpau, J., R. Gardner and B. Gardner (1989) “Expression of person, place and instrument in
ASL utterances of children and chimpanzees” In R. Gardner, B. Gardner and T. van Cantfort
(eds.) Teaching Sign Language to Chimpanzees (240–268) State University of New York
Press
Terrace, H. (1979) Nim: A Chimpanzee Who Learned Sign Language Knopf
Animals and human language 25
CHAPTER 3
The sounds of language
I take it you already know
Of tough and bough and cough and dough?
Others may stumble but not you
On hiccough, thorough, lough and through.
Well done! And now you wish, perhaps,
To learn of less familiar traps?
Beware of heard, a dreadful word,
That looks like beard and sounds like bird.
And dead: it's said like bed, not bead -
For goodness sake don't call it “deed”!
Watch out for meat and great and threat
(They rhyme with suite and straight and debt).
T.S.W. quoted in Mackay (1970)
In Chapter 1, we noted some of the basic features of the human vocal tract and
the intricate muscle interlacing in and around the mouth that give humans the ability
to produce a wide range of sounds with great speed. Yet, as they chatter away,
humans do not simply produce a random selection of these sounds. Only certain
sounds are selected on a regular basis as significant for communicative activity. In
order to identify and describe those sounds, we have to slow down the chatter of
everyday talk and focus on each individual sound segment within the stream of
speech. This may seem straightforward, but it is not an easy task.
Phonetics
Fortunately, there is an already established analytic framework for the study of speech
segments that has been developed and refined for over a hundred years and is known
as the International Phonetic Alphabet, or IPA. In this chapter, we will look at how
the symbols of this alphabet can be used to represent both the consonant and vowel
sounds of English words and what physical aspects of the human vocal tract are
involved in the production of those sounds.
The general study of the characteristics of speech sounds is called phonetics. Our
main interest will be in articulatory phonetics, which is the study of how speech sounds
are made, or articulated. Other areas of study are acoustic phonetics, which deals with
the physical properties of speech as sound waves in the air, and auditory phonetics (or
perceptual phonetics), which deals with the perception, via the ear, of speech sounds.
Voiced and voiceless sounds
In articulatory phonetics, we investigate how speech sounds are produced using the
fairly complex oral equipment we have. We start with the air pushed out by the lungs
up through the trachea (or windpipe) to the larynx. Inside the larynx are your vocal
folds (or vocal cords), which take two basic positions.
1 When the vocal folds are spread apart, the air from the lungs passes between them
unimpeded. Sounds produced in this way are described as voiceless.
2 When the vocal folds are drawn together, the air from the lungs repeatedly pushes
them apart as it passes through, creating a vibration effect. Sounds produced in this
way are described as voiced.
The distinction can be felt physically if you place a fingertip gently on the top of your
Adam’s apple (i.e. that part of your larynx you can feel in your neck below your chin),
then produce sounds such as Z-Z-Z-Z or V-V-V-V. Because these are voiced sounds, you
should be able to feel some vibration. Keeping your fingertip in the same position, now
make the sounds S-S-S-S or F-F-F-F. Because these are voiceless sounds, there should
be no vibration. Another trick is to put a finger in each ear, not too far, and produce the
voiced sounds (e.g. Z-Z-Z-Z) to hear and feel some vibration, whereas no vibration
will be heard or felt if you make voiceless sounds (e.g. S-S-S-S) in the same way.
Place of articulation
Once the air has passed through the larynx, it comes up and out through the mouth
and/or the nose. Most consonant sounds are produced by using the tongue and other
parts of the mouth to constrict, in some way, the shape of the oral cavity through
which the air is passing. The terms used to describe many sounds are those that
The sounds of language 27
denote the place of articulation of the sound: that is, the location inside the mouth at
which the constriction takes place.
What we need is a slice of head. If you crack a head right down the middle, you
will be able to see those parts of the oral cavity that are crucially involved in speech
production. In Figure 3.1, in addition to lips and teeth, a number of other physical
features are identified. To describe the place of articulation of most consonant sounds,
we can start at the front of the mouth and work back. We can also keep the voiced–
voiceless distinction in mind and begin using the symbols of the IPA for specific
sounds. These symbols will be enclosed within square brackets [].
Consonants
Familiar symbols
Many of the symbols used to describe consonant sounds will be familiar. We use [p]
for the consonant in pop, [b] in Bob, and [m] in mom. These are bilabial consonants,
made with both lips. We use [f] and [v] for the labiodentals (using upper teeth and
lower lip) at the beginning and end of five. Behind the upper teeth is a rough area (the
alveolar ridge) where we make the alveolar sounds of [t] in tot, [d] in dad, [s] and [z]
in size, and [n] in nun.
Of course, there isn’t always a match between written letters and phonetic
symbols, as in the pronunciation of the sound at the beginning of photo and the end
nasal cavity
palate
velum
uvula
pharynx
larynx vocal folds
tongue
alveolar ridge
Figure 3.1
28 The Study of Language
of enough. In both cases, we would represent the sound with [f]. More tricky are the
final sounds in the pairs face versus phase and race versus raise: if you listen carefully,
you will hear [s] in the first word of each pair and [z] in the second.
Unfamiliar symbols
Other symbols are much less familiar, as in the two ways of representing the “th”
sounds in English. We use [θ], called “theta,” for the voiceless version, as in three,
wrath. We use [ð], called “eth,” for the voiced version, as in thus, loathe. Because the
teeth are involved in the production of these sounds, they are called dentals, or in
those cases where the tongue tip is between (¼ inter) the teeth, they may be described as interdentals.
There are some special symbols used for the sounds made in the middle area of
the mouth, involving the tongue and the palate (the roof of the mouth). We use [ʃ] for
the “sh” sound, as in shout, shoe-brush, and [ʧ] for the “ch” sound, as in child, church.
These are voiceless.
Their voiced counterparts are [ʒ] for the sound in treasure, rouge, and [ʤ] for the
sound in judge, George. Another voiced sound made in this area is [j], which typically
represents the “y” sound, as in yes, yoyo. Because the palate area is involved in these
sounds, they are described as palatals.
The sounds produced toward the back of the mouth, involving the velum,
are represented by the velars [k], as in kick, and [ɡ], as in gag. Note that phonetic
[ɡ] is different from typewritten “g.” We often use [k] to represent the sound of words
beginning with “c,” as well as some other letters, as in cat, character and queue.
One other consonant produced in this area is [ŋ], called “angma,” as in thong, ringing. Be careful not to be misled by the spelling because both bang and tongue end
with [ŋ] only. There is no [ɡ] sound at the end of these words. Adescription of the place of articulation for each consonant is presented in Table 3.1.
Consonants: manner of articulation
In Table 3.1, there is a detailed analysis of the place of articulation for consonants. From
this we can see that [t] and [s] are similar in that they are both voiceless alveolars. But
they’re clearly different. The difference is in how they are pronounced, or their manner
of articulation. The [t] sound is a “stop” consonant and the [s] sound is a “fricative.”
Stops
In producing a stop consonant, we block the airflow briefly, then let it go abruptly.
The voiceless stops are [p], [t], [k] and the voiced stops are [b], [d], [ɡ]. So, the word
pet begins and ends with voiceless stops and bed with voiced stops.
The sounds of language 29
Fricatives
To produce a fricative, we almost block the airflow and force it through a narrow gap,
creating a type of friction. The voiceless forms are [f], [θ], [s], [ʃ], [h], so that the word
fish begins and ends with voiceless fricatives. The voiced versions are [v], [ð], [z], [ʒ],
so the word those begins and ends with voiced fricatives.