7/14/2018 University of Phoenix: Development Through the Lifespan
https://phoenix.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781323102527/cfi/6/38!/4/2@0:0 1/100
PRINTED BY: lbethune82@email.phoenix.edu. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted.
chapter 6 Emotional and Social Development in Infancy and Toddlerhood
This father takes time to build a strong, affectionate bond with his infant daughter. His warmth and sensitivity engender a sense of security in the baby—a vital foundation for all aspects of early development.
182
7/14/2018 University of Phoenix: Development Through the Lifespan
https://phoenix.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781323102527/cfi/6/38!/4/2@0:0 2/100
PRINTED BY: lbethune82@email.phoenix.edu. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted.
chapter outline
Erikson’s Theory of Infant and Toddler Personality
Basic Trust versus Mistrust
Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt
Emotional Development
Development of Basic Emotions
Understanding and Responding to the Emotions of Others
Emergence of Self-Conscious Emotions
Beginnings of Emotional Self-Regulation
■ BIOLOGY AND ENVIRONMENT Parental Depression and Child Development
Temperament and Development
The Structure of Temperament
Measuring Temperament
Stability of Temperament
Genetic and Environmental Influences
Temperament and Child Rearing: The Goodness-of-Fit Model
■ BIOLOGY AND ENVIRONMENT Development of Shyness and Sociability
Development of Attachment
Bowlby’s Ethological Theory
Measuring the Security of Attachment
Stability of Attachment
Cultural Variations
Factors That Affect Attachment Security
Multiple Attachments
Attachment and Later Development
■ SOCIAL ISSUES: HEALTH Does Child Care in Infancy Threaten Attachment Security and
Later Adjustment?
■ CULTURAL INFLUENCES The Powerful Role of Paternal Warmth in Development
Self-Development
Self-Awareness
182
183
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17335
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17345
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17360
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17374
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17396
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17517
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17553
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17575
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17438
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17631
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17653
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17785
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17812
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17891
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17936
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid17824
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18004
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18028
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18089
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18249
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18267
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18295
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18415
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18615
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18437
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18517
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18654
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18662
7/14/2018 University of Phoenix: Development Through the Lifespan
https://phoenix.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781323102527/cfi/6/38!/4/2@0:0 3/100
Categorizing the Self
Self-Control
As Caitlin reached 8 months of age, her parents noticed that she had become more fearful.
One evening, when Carolyn and David left her with a babysitter, she wailed as they headed
for the door—an experience she had accepted easily a few weeks earlier. Caitlin and
Timmy’s caregiver Ginette also observed an increasing wariness of strangers. When she
turned to go to another room, both babies dropped their play to crawl after her. At the mail
carrier’s knock at the door, they clung to Ginette’s legs, reaching out to be picked up.
At the same time, each baby seemed more willful. Removing an object from the hand
produced little response at 5 months. But at 8 months, when Timmy’s mother, Vanessa, took
away a table knife he had managed to reach, Timmy burst into angry screams and could
not be consoled or distracted.
All Monica and Kevin knew about Grace’s first year was that she had been deeply loved by
her destitute, homeless mother. Separation from her, followed by a long journey to an
unfamiliar home, had left Grace in shock. At first she was extremely sad, turning away
when Monica or Kevin picked her up. But as Grace’s new parents held her close, spoke
gently, and satisfied her craving for food, Grace returned their affection. Two weeks after
her arrival, her despondency gave way to a sunny, easygoing disposition. She burst into a
wide grin, reached out at the sight of Monica and Kevin, and laughed at her brother Eli’s
funny faces. As her second birthday approached, she pointed to herself, exclaiming
“Gwace!” and laid claim to treasured possessions. “Gwace’s chicken!” she would announce
at mealtimes, sucking the marrow from the drumstick, a practice she had brought with her
from Cambodia.
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18741
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_018.xhtml#eid18755
7/14/2018 University of Phoenix: Development Through the Lifespan
https://phoenix.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781323102527/cfi/6/38!/4/2@0:0 4/100
Taken together, the children’s reactions reflect two related aspects of personality
development during the first two years: close ties to others and a sense of self. We begin
with Erikson’s psychosocial theory, which provides an overview of personality
development during infancy and toddlerhood. Then, as we chart the course of emotional
development, we will discover why fear and anger became more apparent in Caitlin’s and
Timmy’s range of emotions by the end of the first year. Our attention then turns to
individual
7/14/2018 University of Phoenix: Development Through the Lifespan
https://phoenix.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781323102527/cfi/6/38!/4/2@0:0 5/100
PRINTED BY: lbethune82@email.phoenix.edu. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted.
differences in temperament. We will examine genetic and environmental contributions to
these differences and their consequences for future development.
Next, we take up attachment to the caregiver, the child’s first affectionate tie. We will see
how the feelings of security that grow out of this important bond support the child’s sense
of independence and expanding social relationships.
Finally, we focus on early self-development. By the end of toddlerhood, Grace recognized
herself in mirrors and photographs, labeled herself as a girl, and showed the beginnings of
self-control. “Don’t touch!” she instructed herself one day as she resisted the desire to pull a
lamp cord out of its socket. Cognitive advances combine with social experiences to produce
these changes during the second year.
Erikson’s Theory of Infant and Toddler Personality
Our discussion of major theories in Chapter 1 revealed that the psychoanalytic perspective
is no longer in the mainstream of human development research. But one of its lasting
contributions is its ability to capture the essence of personality during each period of
development. Recall that Sigmund Freud believed that psychological health and
maladjustment could be traced to the quality of the child’s relationships with parents during
the early years. Although Freud’s preoccupation with the channeling of biological drives
and his neglect of important experiences beyond infancy and early childhood came to be
heavily criticized, the basic outlines of his theory were accepted and elaborated in several
subsequent theories. The most influential is Erik Erikson’s psychosocial theory, also
introduced in Chapter 1.
Basic Trust versus Mistrust
Erikson accepted Freud’s emphasis on the importance of the parent–infant relationship
during feeding, but he expanded and enriched Freud’s view. A healthy outcome during
infancy, Erikson believed, does not depend on the amount of food or oral stimulation
offered but rather on the quality of caregiving: relieving discomfort promptly and
sensitively, holding the infant gently, waiting patiently until the baby has had enough milk,
and weaning when the infant shows less interest in breast or bottle.
183
184
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_011.xhtml#eid5739
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_011.xhtml#eid5739
7/14/2018 University of Phoenix: Development Through the Lifespan
https://phoenix.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781323102527/cfi/6/38!/4/2@0:0 6/100
Erikson recognized that no parent can be perfectly in tune with the baby’s needs. Many
factors affect parental responsiveness—personal happiness, current life conditions (for
example, additional young children in the family), and culturally valued child-rearing
practices. But when the balance of care is sympathetic and loving, the psychological conflict
of the first year—basic trust versus mistrust—is resolved on the positive side. The trusting
infant expects the world to be good and gratifying, so he feels confident about venturing out
and exploring it. The mistrustful baby cannot count on the kindness and compassion of
others, so she protects herself by withdrawing from people and things around her.
Autonomy versus Shame and Doubt
With the transition to toddlerhood, Freud viewed the parents’ manner of toilet training as
decisive for psychological health. In Erikson’ view, toilet training is only one of many
influential experiences. The familiar refrains of newly walking, talking toddlers—“No!” “Do
it myself!”—reveal that they have entered a period of budding selfhood. They want to
decide for themselves, not just in toileting but also in other situations. The conflict of
toddlerhood, autonomy versus shame and doubt, is resolved favorably when parents
provide young children with suitable guidance and reasonable choices. A self-confident,
secure 2-year-old has parents who do not criticize or attack him when he fails at new skills
—using the toilet, eating with a spoon, or putting away toys. And they meet his assertions of
independence with tolerance and understanding—for example, by giving him an extra five
minutes to finish his play before leaving for the grocery store. In contrast, when parents are
over- or undercontrolling, the outcome is a child who feels forced and shamed and who
doubts his ability to control his impulses and act competently on his own.
In sum, basic trust and autonomy grow out of warm, sensitive parenting and reasonable
expectations for impulse control starting in the second year. If children emerge from the
first few years without sufficient trust in caregivers and without a healthy sense of
individuality, the seeds are sown for adjustment problems. Adults who have difficulty
establishing intimate ties, who are overly dependent on a loved one, or who continually
doubt their own ability to meet new challenges may not have fully mastered the tasks of
trust and autonomy during infancy and toddlerhood.
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_040.xhtml#eid48076
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_040.xhtml#eid48018
7/14/2018 University of Phoenix: Development Through the Lifespan
https://phoenix.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781323102527/cfi/6/38!/4/2@0:0 7/100
On a visit to a science museum, a 2-year-old insists on exploring a flight simulator. As the mother supports her toddler’s desire to “do it myself,” she fosters a healthy sense of autonomy.
7/14/2018 University of Phoenix: Development Through the Lifespan
https://phoenix.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781323102527/cfi/6/38!/4/2@0:0 8/100
PRINTED BY: lbethune82@email.phoenix.edu. Printing is for personal, private use only. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted without publisher's prior permission. Violators will be prosecuted.
Emotional Development
TAKE A MOMENT… Observe several infants and toddlers, noting the emotions each displays,
the cues you rely on to interpret the baby’s emotional state, and how caregivers respond.
Researchers have conducted many such observations to find out how babies convey their
emotions and interpret those of others. They have discovered that emotions play powerful
roles in organizing the attainments that Erikson regarded as so important: social
relationships, exploration of the environment, and discovery of the self (Halle, 2003; Saarni
et al., 2006).
Think back to the dynamic systems perspective introduced in Chapters 1 and 4. As you read
about early emotional development in the sections that follow, notice how emotions are an
integral part of young children’s dynamic systems of action. Emotions energize
development. At the same time, they are an aspect of the system that develops, becoming
more varied and complex as children reorganize their behavior to attain new goals
(Campos, Frankel, & Camras, 2004; Thompson, Winer, & Goodvin, 2011).
Because infants cannot describe their feelings, determining exactly which emotions they are
experiencing is a challenge. Cross-cultural evidence reveals that people around the world
associate photographs of different facial expressions with emotions in the same way
(Ekman, 2003; Ekman & Friesen, 1972). These findings inspired researchers to analyze
infants’ facial patterns to determine the range of emotions they display at different ages. But
to express a particular emotion, infants, children, and adults actually use diverse responses
—not just facial expressions but also vocalizations and body movements—which vary with
their developing capacities, goals, and contexts. Therefore, to infer babies’ emotions as
accurately as possible, researchers are best off attending to multiple interacting behavioral
cues and seeing how they vary across situations believed to elicit different emotions (Lewis,
2000, 2008).
Development of Basic Emotions
Basic emotions—happiness, interest, surprise, fear, anger, sadness, and disgust—are
universal in humans and other primates and have a long evolutionary history of promoting
survival. Do infants come into the world with the ability to express basic emotions?
184
185
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid57270
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid64191
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_011.xhtml#eid5739
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_016.xhtml#eid13069
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid53291
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid66160
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid55320
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid55323
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid60082
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid51171
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_040.xhtml#eid48070
7/14/2018 University of Phoenix: Development Through the Lifespan
https://phoenix.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781323102527/cfi/6/38!/4/2@0:0 9/100
Although signs of some emotions are present, babies’ earliest emotional life consists of little
more than two global arousal states: attraction to pleasant stimulation and withdrawal from
unpleasant stimulation (Camras et al., 2003; Fox, 1991). Only gradually do emotions become
clear, well-organized signals.
According to one view, sensitive, contingent caregiver communication, in which parents
selectively mirror aspects of the baby’s diffuse emotional behavior, helps infants construct
emotional expressions that more closely resemble those of adults (Gergely & Watson, 1999).
With age, face, voice, and posture start to form organized patterns that vary meaningfully
with environmental events. For example, Caitlin typically responded to her parents’ playful
interaction with a joyful face, pleasant babbling, and a relaxed posture, as if to say, “This is
fun!” In contrast, an unresponsive parent often evokes a sad face, fussy sounds, and a
drooping body (sending the message, “I’m despondent”) or an angry face, crying, and “pick-
me-up” gestures (as if to say, “Change this unpleasant event!”) (Weinberg & Tronick, 1994;
Yale et al., 1999). Gradually, emotional expressions become well-organized and specific—
and therefore provide more precise information about the baby’s internal state.
Four basic emotions—happiness, anger, sadness, and fear—have received the most research
attention. Let’s see how they develop.
Happiness.
Happiness—expressed first in blissful smiles and later through exuberant laughter—
contributes to many aspects of development. When infants achieve new skills, they smile
and laugh, displaying delight in motor and cognitive mastery. As the smile encourages
caregivers to be affectionate and stimulating, the baby smiles even more (Aksan &
Kochanska, 2004). Happiness binds parent and baby into a warm, supportive relationship
that fosters the infant’s developing competencies.
During the early weeks, newborn babies smile when full, during REM sleep, and in response
to gentle touches and sounds, such as stroking of the skin, rocking, and the mother’s soft,
high-pitched voice. By the end of the first month, infants smile at dynamic, eye-catching
sights, such as a bright object jumping suddenly across their field of vision. Between 6 and
10 weeks, the parent’s communication evokes a broad grin called the social smile (Lavelli &
Fogel, 2005; Sroufe & Waters, 1976). These changes parallel the development of infant
perceptual capacities—in particular, sensitivity to visual patterns, including the human face
(see Chapter 4). And social smiling becomes better-organized and stable as babies learn to
use it to evoke and sustain pleasurable face-to-face interaction.
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid53300
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid56073
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid56564
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid67196
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid67792
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid51321
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_040.xhtml#eid50740
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid59822
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_041.xhtml#eid65407
https://jigsaw.vitalsource.com/books/9781323102527/epub/OPS/loc_016.xhtml#eid13069
7/14/2018 University of Phoenix: Development Through the Lifespan
https://phoenix.vitalsource.com/#/books/9781323102527/cfi/6/38!/4/2@0:0 10/100
Laughter, which appears around 3 to 4 months, reflects faster processing of information
than smiling. But, as with smiling, the first laughs occur in response to very active stimuli,
such as the parent saying playfully, “I’m gonna get you!” and kissing the baby’s tummy. As
infants understand more about their world, they laugh at events with subtler elements of
surprise, such as a silent game of peekaboo (Sroufe & Wunsch, 1972).
Around the middle of the first year, infants smile and laugh more when interacting with
familiar people, a preference that strengthens the parent–child bond. Between 8 and 10
months, infants more often interrupt their play with an interesting toy to relay their delight
to an attentive adult (Venezia et al., 2004). And like adults, 10- to 12-month-olds have several
smiles, which vary with context—a broad, “cheek-raised” smile in response to a parent’s
greeting; a reserved, muted smile for a friendly stranger; and a “mouth-open” smile during
stimulating play (Bolzani et al., 2002; Messinger & Fogel, 2007). By the end of the first year,
the smile has become a deliberate social signal.
Anger and Sadness.
Newborn babies respond with generalized distress to a variety of unpleasant experiences,
including hunger, painful medical procedures, changes in body temperature, and too much
or too little stimulation. From 4 to 6 months into the second year, angry expressions
increase in frequency and intensity (Braungart-Rieker, Hill-Soderlund, & Karrass, 2010).
Older infants also react with anger in a wider range of situations—when an interesting
object or event is removed, an expected pleasant event does not occur, their arms are
restrained, the caregiver leaves for a brief time, or they are put down for a nap (Camras et
al., 1992; Stenberg & Campos, 1990; Sullivan & Lewis, 2003).